- 1Department of Occupational and Environmental Health, Unisante, Université de Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
- 2Department of Psychiatry, Psychiatric Epidemiology and Psychopathology Research Center, Lausanne University Hospital, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
- 3Department of Consultation-Liaison Psychiatry and Psychosomatic Medicine, University Hospital Zürich, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
Objectives: To prospectively investigate the association between Effort-Reward Imbalance (ERI) and over-commitment and the scores of the burnout dimensions over a 4 years follow-up period considering potential confounders.
Methods: Data stemmed from CoLaus|PsyCoLaus, a population-based cohort study including 575 participants (mean age 55 years, 50% men). Participants completed the Maslach Burnout Inventory-General Survey, ERI and over-commitment questionnaires at baseline (T1) and after a 4 years follow-up (T2), and provided demographic, behavioral, psychiatric, personality and social support information through self-reported questionnaires and semi-structured interviews. Serially adjusted linear regression models were used.
Results: ERI and over-commitment were not associated longitudinally with any of the burnout dimensions when controlling for confounders. One standard deviation increases in the scores of exhaustion, cynicism and professional efficacy were associated with one standard deviation increase in the scores of the same burnout dimensions longitudinally, and these associations were independent of the effects of ERI and over-commitment.
Conclusion: Future studies should re-examine the effect of ERI and over-commitment on workers’ burnout, considering the effects of confounders.
Introduction
The changes in working conditions brought on by technology advancements [1] have led to an increase in work stress, which has become a growing public health concern [2]. Work stress is defined by World Health Organization (WHO) as the reaction of workers to job demands that exceed their knowledge and abilities [3]. The measurement of work stress can be divided into three approaches: 1-assessment of stressors reported by workers, 2-assessment of responses to stress (job strain such as burnout) and 3- assessment of the experience of work stress [4]. The Effort-Reward Imbalance (ERI) reflects the exposure to work stressors such as high work demands justifying employee’s efforts to manage these efforts and reward or recognition of these efforts. The ERI and over-commitment model allows assessing the impact of these exposures on health by examining the imbalance between job demands (extrinsic and intrinsic) and rewards [5]. The ERI model has been shown to be useful for explaining and preventing work-related stress in a systematic review [6] and has been linked to the development of stress-related disorders through pathways such as altered heart rate variability, blood lipids, and metabolic syndrome [7]. Chronic work stress can additionally lead to occupational burnout [8].
Theoretical approaches to burnout can be classified into four categories: individual, interpersonal, organizational, and societal [9]. The interpersonal model suggests that burnout occurs in three phases: exhaustion from job demands, detachment (cynicism), and reduced professional efficacy [10]. The Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI), a widely used tool for burnout assessment [11, 12], is based on this model. The WHO has recognized burnout as a factor influencing health status in its 11th revision of the international classification of diseases [13] that is linked to both psychological and physical consequences [14]. Although burnout is often described a response to chronic work stressors, it is distinct from work stress [15, 16]. Following the previously mentioned three approaches for measurement of stress at work, ERI could be the stressors reported by workers whereas burnout is the response to these stressors (i.e., strain).
The evidence on the relevance of the ERI model for research on burnout etiology is currently limited, with three reviews [16–18], and 15 original studies, of which 12 were cross-sectional [19–30] and three were prospective cohorts [31–33]. The limitations of cross-sectional studies in determining the temporal relationships between variables and causality between exposure and effect [34], as well as the scarcity of cohort studies are two major gaps in the literature. The cross-sectional design may result in oversimplification and misinterpretation of the relationship between the exposure to work-related stress and burnout, particularly if the impact of confounding factors is not taken into consideration [35]. The selection of covariates was guided by available literature that reports their associations with burnout [17, 18]. We identified two systematic reviews that concluded a negative correlation between age and burnout symptoms [19, 20]. Another meta-analysis revealed a slight inclination for women to experience emotional exhaustion, while men tend to exhibit depersonalization more frequently [21]. The Aronsson et al., meta-analysis recommended including smoking as a covariate when analyzing burnout studies [22]. Notably, a meta-analysis demonstrated a significant association between burnout and depression (r = 0.52) as well as burnout and anxiety (r = 0.46) [23]. Further insights were gained from a study of the Finnish general working population, where burnout was notably correlated with alcohol dependence [24]. Personality treats such as neuroticism and extraversion were linked to increased exhaustion (rho = 0.33 and 0.13, respectively) in a meta-analysis based on three and one study(s), respectively [18]. Additionally, a systematic review underscored the significance of support from both supervisors and coworkers in preventing burnout among nurses [25]. Higher effort-reward imbalance was reported to be correlated with higher age [26], in men compared to women [27], in smokers compared to non-smokers [28], with less physical activity [29], and more alcohol use [30]. ERI was also reported to predict affective disorders [31] in general [31] and depression specifically [32]. Higher ERI was correlated with lower social support [33] and extraversion and increased neuroticism [34] were also reported in the literature.
Therefore, the impact of these variables on the relationship between dimensions of the ERI model and occupational burnout requires careful consideration.
A third gap in the literature comes with the inconsistencies in previous findings of the three prospective cohort studies mentioned above. Hadžibajramović et al. established a significant relationship between effort, reward, their joint exposure and burnout, after controlling for potential confounding factors such as social support, physical activity, age, and time [35]. Leineweber et al. observed significant direct paths between ERI and exhaustion, as measured three times with a 2 years interval and controlled for age, sex, income, and job change [36]. Conversely, Nuebling et al. found that only reward predicted burnout after adjusting for age, sex, socioeconomic status, and baseline burnout [37].
Therefore, the aims of this study were to 1) investigate the prospective association between the exposure to work-related stress at baseline, modeled as effort, reward and their combined effect, over-commitment, and the three burnout dimensions: exhaustion, cynicism, and professional efficacy across a 4 years follow-up; and 2) test the potentially confounding effects of variables, rigorously identified in the published literature. The Swiss prospective population-based cohort Colaus|PsyCoLaus [38, 39] provides a robust methodological approach to address the limitations of previous studies in investigating the association between the dimensions of the ERI, over-commitment, and occupational burnout, while controlling for potential confounding factors.
We propose six hypotheses:
1) Higher effort-reward imbalance, effort, and over-commitment will be associated with an increase in exhaustion and cynicism longitudinally.
2) Higher reward will be associated with a decrease in exhaustion and cynicism longitudinally.
3) Higher effort-reward imbalance, effort, and over-commitment will be associated with lower professional efficacy longitudinally.
4) Higher reward will be associated with an increase in professional efficacy longitudinally.
5) High scores in burnout dimensions will be associated with increased scores of the same burnout dimensions longitudinally.
6) The associations between effort, reward and their combined effect, over-commitment, and the three burnout dimensions exhaustion, cynicism, and professional efficacy, will decrease after adjustment for potentially confounding effects of other variables.
Methods
Study Sample and Follow-Up
The present data stem from the prospective cohort study CoLaus|PsyCoLaus, designed to assess cardiovascular risk factors and mental disorders in the community as well as their associations. The methodological features of the recruitment and baseline assessments of CoLaus|PsyCoLaus have been described in detail elsewhere [38, 39]. Briefly, CoLaus|PsyCoLaus includes a random sample of 6,734 participants (age range: 35–75 years) selected from the general population according to the civil register of the city of Lausanne (Switzerland). After a first physical and psychiatric investigation, which took place between 2003 and 2008, the cohort was followed-up for approximately 5 (first follow-up, FU1), 9 (second follow-up, FU2), and 13 years (third follow-up, FU3). Burnout, ERI and over-commitment were only measured at FU2 (first assessment, called T1) and FU3 (second assessment, called T2), the analysis was thus restricted to these two follow-ups (Figure 1). From T1 onward, current workers and participants without occupational activity for a maximum 1 year were invited to fill in self-reported questionnaires on ERI, over-commitment and burnout during the psychiatric evaluation. A total of 1,336 participants reported occupational activity at T1 and occupational activity or retirement for less than 1 year at T2. Among them, 608 participants completed burnout and ERI questionnaires at both T1 and T2. Participants received the self-administered questionnaire during the psychiatric assessment. Two reminders were sent if they did not return the questionnaire. A total of 33 participants had incomplete data on potential confounders and were excluded, resulting in a final sample of 575 participants (Figure 2). A description of the sample is provided in Table 1. Only 6.4% changed their occupational activities between the first (T1) and the second (T2) assessment. The mean (SD) age of the participants was 54.7 (5.3) years and 50% were men. Almost half of the participants were former smokers; the majority reported to be physically active for at least once a week. About half of the participants had Major Depressive Disorders (MDD) or suffered from depression in the past and one-fourth with an anxiety disorder or suffered from anxiety in the past.
Figure 1. CoLaus|PsyCoLaus follow-ups and variables assessed at each follow-up (CoLaus|PsyCoLaus, Lausanne, Switzerland 2003–2020).
Figure 2. Flow chart of the study for the associations between Effort-Reward Imbalance and occupational burnout in a 4 years follow-up (CoLaus|PsyCoLaus, Lausanne, Switzerland 2003–2020). FU2, follow-up 2; FU3, follow-up 3. T1, first assessment, T2, second assessment.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the study variables among participants (CoLaus|PsyCoLaus, Lausanne, Switzerland 2003–2020).
Measurement of Study Variables
Effort-Reward Imbalance, Over-Commitment, and Burnout
Siegrist proposed a model to assess the exposure to work-related stress through an extrinsic component which is ERI and an intrinsic component which is over-commitment [40]. ERI and over-commitment were measured using the French validated version [41] of Siegrist’s Job Stress Questionnaire [40]. This questionnaire consists of 23 items split in three subscales: effort (6 items), reward [i.e., esteem (4 items), promotion (5 items), and security (2 items)], and overcommitment (6 items). Examples of items are “Over the past few years, my job has become more and more demanding” from the effort subscale, “Considering all my efforts and achievements, my salary/income is adequate” from the reward subscale and “At work, I am often pressed for time” from the over-commitment subscale. Each item can be scored on a 5-point Likert scale (i.e., from 1 “Strongly disagree” to 4 “Strongly agree”) in the effort and reward subscales, and on a 5-point Likert scale in the over-commitment subscale. The effort/reward ratio is calculated using the formula: effort/reward*correction factor, where the correction factor (0.55) adjusts for the unequal number of items of the effort and reward scores [42]. The Cronbach’s alphas of all subscales were reported to be higher than 0.70: 0.75 for the effort subscale, 0.86 (for men) and 0.88 (for women) for the reward subscale and 0.82 for the over-commitment subscale.
Burnout was measured using the French validated version [43] of the Maslach Burnout Inventory General Survey (MBI-GS) [44]. The 16 items of the MBI-GS are scored on a 7-point Likert-type scale from 0 “never” to 6 “every day”. This questionnaire consists of two negatively worded subscales; exhaustion (5 items), and cynicism (5 items) and one positively worded subscale; professional efficacy (6 items). Examples of items are “I feel burned out from my work” from the exhaustion subscale, “I would just like to do my job without being disturbed” from the cynicism subscale and “I think I am good at my job” from the professional efficacy subscale. The Cronbach’s alphas of all subscales were reported to be higher than 0.70: 0.90 for the exhaustion subscale, 0.86 for the cynicism subscale and 0.87 for the professional efficacy subscale.
Potential Confounders
Diagnostic information on mental disorders was collected using the French version [45] of the semi-structured Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies (DIGS) [46]. The French version of this instrument has adequate inter-rater and test-retest reliability for major mood [47] and substance use disorders [48]. The DIGS was completed with anxiety disorder sections of the French version [49] of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia-Lifetime and Anxiety disorder version (SADS-LA) [50]. Diagnoses were assigned according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) [51]. The DIGS also collects information on smoking status and physical activity (at least once a week). Interviewers were master-level psychologists, who were trained over a 1–2 months period. Each interview and diagnostic assignment was reviewed by an experienced senior psychologist.
The personality dimensions of Neuroticism and Extraversion were assessed using the French validated version [52] of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ) [53]. Social support from family, friends, and significant others was measured using the French version [54] of the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS) [55].
The potential confounders were age, sex, physical activity, smoking status, psychiatric disorders [major depressive disorder, anxiety disorders (generalized anxiety disorder, panic disorder, agoraphobia, social phobia), illicit drug and alcohol use disorders (abuse or dependence)], personality traits (neuroticism and extraversion), and social support (from friends, family and significant others).
Statistical Analysis
Data descriptive for demographic feature, health behaviors, psychiatric disorders, personality traits and social support were used to characterize the sample. In order to assess the associations between the ERI and over-commitment dimensions at T1 and the score of the three burnout dimensions (exhaustion, cynicism, professional efficacy) at T2, two separate sets of serially adjusted linear regression models were run: one set of models included the effort-reward ratio and the over-commitment baseline dimensions as independent variables, the other set of models included the ERI baseline dimensions [effort, the three reward sub-dimensions (esteem, promotion and security)] as well as the over-commitment baseline dimension as independent variables. The scores of the three burnout dimensions at T2 were the dependent variables. The first model (Model 1) was adjusted for the respective burnout dimension at T1. Model 2 was additionally adjusted for age, sex, the follow-up duration and the two other burnout dimension scores at T1. The fully adjusted model (Model 3) was the Model 2 further adjusted for potential confounders at T1 [physical activity, smoking status, psychiatric disorders (major depressive disorder, anxiety disorders (generalized anxiety disorder, panic disorder, agoraphobia, social phobia), illicit drug and alcohol use disorders (abuse or dependence)), personality traits, and social support]. We computed standardized beta coefficients by transforming all continuous variables to z-scores. Prior to conducting our analysis, we assessed for multicollinearity among the variables and found that none of the correlations exceeded a threshold of r = 0.70 [56], and the highest observed correlation was 0.62 (Supplementary Table S1). We performed paired t-tests to compare the scores of burnout dimensions between T1 and T2. The missing data in this study was 5% for all variables including confounders. Multiple imputation provides negligible benefit in this case [57, 58], we thus decided to perform a list-wise exclusion and complete case analysis. All statistical analyses were performed using Stata statistical software version 16 [59] and a p-value <0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Results
The Associations Between Effort-Reward Imbalance, Over-Commitment, and the Dimensions of Burnout
Using the first set of models with the effort-reward ratio and the over-commitment dimension at the first assessment as independent variables, no significant associations were found between these scores and the three burnout dimensions scores at T2 (Models 1, 2, 3, Table 2). Using the second set of models with the ERI (Effort, esteem, promotion, security) and the over-commitment dimensions at the first assessment as independent variables, one standard deviation increase in promotion scores at T1 was significantly associated with one standard deviation decrease in the scores of exhaustion at T2 (Model 1, Table 2). Nevertheless, this association became non-significant in Models 2 and 3 after adjustment for potential confounders. One standard deviation increase in the scores of security and promotion at T1 were associated with one standard deviation decrease in the scores of cynicism at T2 in Models 1 and 2. However, these associations became non-significant in the fully adjusted model (Model 3, Table 2) taking into account health behaviors, psychiatric disorder, personality traits and social support. Effort, esteem and over-commitment dimensions were not associated with any scores of burnout dimensions in this second set of analyses. The Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) for the models 1, 2, and 3 indicated that Model 3 performance is the most preferable for exhaustion and cynicism whereas Model 2 performance is the most preferable for professional efficacy (Table 2).
Table 2. Linear regression models of the associations between the Effort-Reward Imbalance (ERI) dimensions, effort-reward ratio, and over-commitment at the first assessment and the scores of burnout dimensions at the second assessment with 4 year follow-up period (n = 575) (CoLaus|PsyCoLaus, Lausanne, Switzerland 2003–2020).
The Associations Between Potential Confounders and Burnout
The results of paired t-test indicated that the score of exhaustion (p = 0.004) and cynicism (p = 0.02) were lower at T1 compared to T2. The scores of professional efficacy were not statically different (p = 0.45). In the fully adjusted models (Models 3) including either the effort-reward ratio and the over-commitment dimensions or the ERI (effort, esteem, promotion, security) and the over-commitment dimensions at the first assessment as independent variables, one standard deviation increase in age was associated with a one standard deviation decrease in exhaustion scores at T2 while one standard deviation increase in neuroticism score was associated with a one standard deviation increase in exhaustion scores at T2 (Table 3 and Supplementary Table S2). In addition, when using the effort-reward ratio and the over-commitment dimensions at the first assessment as independent variables, one standard deviation increase in extraversion scores was associated with a one standard deviation decrease in exhaustion scores at the second assessment (Table 3). Similarly, using either the effort-reward ratio and the over-commitment dimensions or the ERI and the over-commitment dimensions at the first assessment as independent variables, being physically active for at least once per week and a one standard deviation increase in extraversion scores at T1 were associated with a one standard deviation decrease in cynicism scores at T2 (Table 3 and Supplementary Table S2). One standard deviation increase in scores of exhaustion, cynicism and professional efficacy at T1 were associated with a one standard deviation increase in the scores of the same burnout dimensions at T2, and these associations were independent of the effects of ERI and over-commitment. In addition, a one standard deviation increase in cynicism score at T1 was associated with a one standard deviation decrease in professional efficacy scores at T2 (Table 3 and Supplementary Table S2). No significant associations between sex, smoking status, lifetime psychiatric disorders (major depressive disorder, substance use disorders, and anxiety disorders), social support (friends, family, significant others) at T1 and the scores of exhaustion, cynicism, and professional efficacy at T2 were found.
Table 3. Fully adjusted linear regression models of the associations between Effort-Reward Imbalance (ERI) dimensions, effort-reward ratio, and over-commitment measured at the first assessment (2014–2018) and the scores of burnout dimensions measured at the second assessment (2018–2021) adjusting for potential covariates (n = 575) (CoLaus|PsyCoLaus, Lausanne, Switzerland 2003–2020).
Discussion
Main Findings
Based on a large population-based prospective cohort, the most salient findings were: 1) a higher score in the reward sub-scale promotion at the first assessment were associated with a decrease in exhaustion and cynicism scores, and a higher security score at the first assessment was associated with a decrease of cynicism score across a 4 years follow-up period with minimal adjustment; 2) after controlling for potential confounding factors, these associations did not reach statistical significance. Age, personality traits (neuroticism and extraversion) and physical activity were associated with exhaustion or cynicism scores across the follow-up period.
The scores of exhaustions and cynicism were higher at the second assessment compared to the first assessment. Since the second assessment was performed between 2018 and 2020, this may be partially explained by COVID-19 pandemic. Indeed, the pandemic increased the exposure to work-related psychosocial factors which led to the increase in burnout. Due to the lack of a valid cut-off for MBI-GS, we could not investigate whether the workers had an increased rate of exhaustion and cynicism, after the 4 years follow-up but they did not develop burnout yet or they developed burnout and continued working.
Finally, a higher score of a burnout dimension at the first assessment was associated with an increase in the score of the same burnout dimension after a 4 years follow-up with the largest effect sizes compared to the independent variables and other confounders. This finding indicates that the changes in the outcomes between the first and the second assessment are largely explained by the outcome scores at the first assessment. In addition, cynicism at the first assessment was associated longitudinally with a decrease in the score of professional efficacy. This finding is in line with the argument stated by Leiter and Maslach that exhaustion is the first burnout symptom to develop, leading then to cynicism or distancing from work as a maladaptive coping strategy, which, if not managed appropriately, can further lead to reduced professional efficacy [60].
Effort-Reward Imbalance, Over-Commitment and Burnout
The results from three previous cohort studies [35–37], in which burnout was measured using the Shirom-Melamed Burnout Measure (SMBM) [61] or the Copenhagen Burnout Inventory (CBI) [62], showed associations between ERI and burnout. However, in the study of Nuebling et al., 2022, only reward remained to be significantly associated with burnout after adjustment for the outcome at baseline although the strength of this association was three-fold smaller. We also controlled for the outcomes at the first assessment in our study and the only statistically significant associations were the one between promotion and exhaustion and the ones between promotion, security and cynicism before controlling for all confounders. The inclusion of a large set of confounders in the fully adjusted models (Model 3), may explain why initially identified associations turned statistically non-significant. The cross-sectional studies previously conducted in Switzerland [63–69], found that the exposure to work-related stress measured using ERI predicts burnout. Some of these studies concluded that the association between ERI and burnout is weaker compared to that with work-life balance [66], work-privacy conflict [68], or neuroticism [63]. In the present study, we also found a significant effect of neuroticism on an increase in exhaustion. This study is the first one conducted in Switzerland that examined the relationship between over-commitment and the dimensions of burnout. However, beyond Switzerland, over-commitment was associated with burnout in two longitudinal studies [37, 70] and with the three burnout dimensions measured using the MBI-GS, in two cross-sectional studies [71, 72]. In the four aforementioned studies, over-commitment was associated with an increase in burnout and in its three dimensions.
The Effects of Other Confounders
Age range may have influenced the results of this study since all participants were older than 47 years. Two systematic reviews concluded that young age is associated with higher rates of burnout symptoms [19, 20, 73]. Among the other confounders, we found a negative association of physical activity with cynicism but not with exhaustion This contradicts the results of prior studies reviewed by [74]. We examined the association between two personality traits (i.e., neuroticism, and extraversion) and burnout. Two cross-sectional studies in Switzerland also found that neuroticism is associated with an increase in burnout while extraversion is associated with a decrease in burnout [63, 64], and the effect size of the association with neuroticism was also larger than the one with extraversion; these findings confirm the results of our study. It is worth mentioning that neuroticism and exhaustion are overlapping constructs, so when both are measured using self-reported questionnaires, negative affectivity may have biased the results [9]. We found no association between social support and burnout, opposite to what was suggested in two out of three cross-sectional studies addressing this question in Switzerland [64, 68]. The differences between the results of this study and those previously cited, may be potentially attributed to the nature of the analysis employed (i.e., longitudinal versus cross-sectional).
It is noteworthy to mention that the prevalence of psychiatric disorders was elevated in this study sample, although this sample is a sample of the general population. The reason for this high prevalence could be clarified by the specific features of the PsyCoLaus sample and the diagnostic instrument used [75]. First, the PsyCoLaus sample was recruited from an urban area (the city of Lausanne) where there is evidence for high prevalence of psychiatric disorder. Second, the features of the diagnostic instrument (DIGS interview) further encouraged the high estimates of psychiatric disorders. Lastly, we cannot rule out that participants with chronic forms of psychiatric disorders were more likely to take part in the study [75].
Strengths and Limitations
The present study is a pioneering cohort study in Switzerland and one of the few in Europe to investigate the longitudinal relationship between the exposure to work-related stress, as modeled by the ERI, over-commitment, and the dimensions of burnout. To control for potential confounding variables, physician-assessed diagnoses of Major Depressive Disorder, anxiety disorders, alcohol use disorder, and drug use disorder were included in the models. Additionally, baseline burnout dimensions scores were controlled for in the analysis of the longitudinal associations over a 4 years follow-up period. However, the study also has some limitations. Self-reported measurement tools were utilized to assess ERI, over-commitment, burnout, support, and personality traits, which may have influenced their results by negative affectivity [76] or social desirability [77]. The source population consisted of the randomly selected residents of Lausanne, Switzerland and therefore, caution should be exercised when generalizing the results to other regions in Switzerland or other countries.
Implication of the Findings
ERI and over-commitment were not associated longitudinally with any of the burnout dimensions when other potentially confounding factors (age, sex, physical activity, smoking status, psychiatric disorders, personality traits, and social support) were taken into account. These findings are different compared to cross-sectional studies or longitudinal ones with no adequate control for confounding factors. While this study has numerous advantages, specifically the certainty of assessment of temporal effects (in contrast to cross-sectional studies), causal assumptions should not be drawn considering there are other important criteria of causality that were not met [78]. The study design implemented in this study is observational and not experimental and thus some criteria could not be tested particularly with merely two measurement points. Nonetheless, prospective cohort studies have the strongest potential to identify a causal risk factor among observational studies.
The findings of this study hold important implications, particularly in the context of the Swiss Federal Office of Statistics’ report from the Swiss Health Survey of 2017, which indicated that the Lake Geneva region had a high prevalence of emotional exhaustion and work stress [73]. Thus, our study adds valuable information on the relationship between the exposure to work-related stress and occupational burnout in the working population of Lausanne.
Practical Implications
Based on the findings of this study we suggest future studies to re-examine the associations between the exposure to work stress and burnout in the activity sectors with high prevalence of emotional exhaustion and work stress [73]. Future studies should also use shorter follow-up periods (less than 1 year) between measurement points and control for known confounders. Burnout latency may be relatively short (i.e., 6–12 months) followed by a potential recovery or uncontrolled bias. Another suggestion could be to implement future studies using shorter follow-up lengths (up to 1 year) between measurement points while taking the effects of confounders into account. Burnout latency may be relatively short based on the finding of a meta-analysis of burnout predictors [18].
The results of this study underline the need for future studies focused on younger workers and targeted interventions to reduce occupational burnout. Additionally, promoting intergenerational collaboration in the workplace may also prove useful in mitigating burnout since older workers may help younger ones in developing adaptive coping strategies. Older workers were shown to have better skills in stress management at work and report less burnout compared to young workers [79, 80].
Ethics Statement
The studies involving humans were approved by the institutional Ethics Committee of the University of Lausanne, which afterwards became the Ethics Commission of the Canton of Vaud (www.cer-vd.ch) approved the baseline CoLaus|PsyColaus study (reference 16/03; 134-03,134-05bis, 134-05-2 to 5 addenda 1 to 4). The approval was renewed for the first (reference 33/09; 239/09), the second (reference 26/14; 239/09 addendum 2), the third (PB_2018-00040; 239/09 addenda 3 to 4) and the forth [PB_2018_0038 (239/09)] follow-ups. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, IG; methodology, YS, SR, M-PS, RvK, MP, and IG; validation, YS, SR, M-PS, RvK, MP, and IG; formal analysis, YS, SR, and M-PS; investigation, YS, SR, and M-PS; resources, YS, SR, and M-PS; data curation, YS, SR, and M-PS; writing—original draft preparation, YS, SR, and M-PS; writing—review and editing, YS, SR, and M-PS; visualization, YS, SR, and M-PS; supervision, IG; project administration, IG; funding acquisition, IG. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
The authors declare that this study received funding from The European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program [under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement No. 801076, through the SSPH+ Global PhD Fellowship Program in Public Health Sciences (GlobalP3HS) of the Swiss School of Public Health] and Unisanté via the General Directorate of Health of the Canton of Vaud via the grant of the Commission for Health Promotion and the Fight against Addictions Grant N◦8273/3636000000-801 to support the PhD position of YS; and GlaxoSmithKline, the Faculty of Biology and Medicine of Lausanne, the Swiss National Science Foundation (grants 3200B0–105993, 3200B0-118308, 33CSCO-122661, 33CS30-139468, 33CS30-148401, 33CS30_177535 and 3247730_204523) and the Swiss Personalized Health Network [project: Swiss Ageing Citizen Reference (grant 2018DRI01)] to support The CoLaus|PsyCoLaus study. The funder was not involved in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article or the decision to submit it for publication.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that they do not have any conflicts of interest.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.ssph-journal.org/articles/10.3389/ijph.2023.1606160/full#supplementary-material
References
1. ILO. Non-Standard Employment Around the World: Understanding Challenges, Shaping Prospects. Geneva, Switzerland: ILO (2016). Office, IL.
2. WHO. Protecting Workers' Health (2017). Available From: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/protecting-workers'-health#:∼:text=Health%20risks%20at%20the%20workplace,workplace%20hierarchy%20also%20affect%20health (Accessed April 21, 2023).
3. WHO. Occupational Health: Stress at the Workplace (2020). Available From: https://www.who.int/news-room/questions-and-answers/item/ccupational-health-stress-at-the-workplace (Accessed April 21, 2023).
4. Stanton, JM, Balzer, WK, Smith, PC, Parra, LF, and Ironson, G. A General Measure of Work Stress: The Stress in General Scale. Educ Psychol Meas (2001) 61(5):866–88. doi:10.1177/00131640121971455
5. Siegrist, J. Adverse Health Effects of High-Effort/Low-Reward Conditions. J Occup Health Psychol (1996) 1(1):27–41. doi:10.1037//1076-8998.1.1.27
6. Siegrist, J, and Li, J. Associations of Extrinsic and Intrinsic Components of Work Stress With Health: A Systematic Review of Evidence on the Effort-Reward Imbalance Model. Int J Environ Res Public Health (2016) 13(4):432. doi:10.3390/ijerph13040432
7. Siegrist, J, and Li, J. Work Stress and Altered Biomarkers: A Synthesis of Findings Based on the Effort-Reward Imbalance Model. Int J Environ Res Public Health (2017) 14(11):1373. doi:10.3390/ijerph14111373
9. Schaufeli, W, and Enzmann, D. The Burnout Companion to Study and Practice: A Critical Analysis. 1st ed. USA: Taylor and Francis (1998).
10. Maslach, C, and Leiter, MP. Understanding the Burnout Experience: Recent Research and Its Implications for Psychiatry. World Psychiatry (2016) 15(2):103–11. doi:10.1002/wps.20311
11. Lubbadeh, T. Job Burnout: A General Literature Review. Int Rev Manage Marketing (2020) 10(3):7–15. doi:10.32479/irmm.9398
12. Shoman, Y, Marca, SC, Bianchi, R, Godderis, L, van der Molen, HF, and Guseva Canu, I. Psychometric Properties of Burnout Measures: A Systematic Review. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci (2021) 30:e8. doi:10.1017/S2045796020001134
13. WHO. ICD-11: International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems. 11th ed. (2019). ICD-11 2022 release.
14. Salvagioni, DAJ, Melanda, FN, Mesas, AE, González, AD, Gabani, FL, and Andrade, SM. Physical, Psychological and Occupational Consequences of Job Burnout: A Systematic Review of Prospective Studies. PLoS One (2017) 12(10):e0185781. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0185781
15. Edú-Valsania, S, Laguía, A, and Moriano, JA. Burnout: A Review of Theory and Measurement. Int J Environ Res Public Health (2022) 19(3):1780. doi:10.3390/ijerph19031780
16. Maslach, C, and Schaufeli, WB. Historical and Conceptual Development of Burnout. In: C SMaslach, WB Schaufeli, and T Marek, editors. Professional Burnout: Recent Developments in Theory and Research USA. USA: Taylor and Francis (1993). p. 1–16.
18. Shoman, Y, Rousson, V, Bianchi, R, and Guseva Canu, I. Holistic Assessment of Factors Associated With Exhaustion, the Main Symptom of Burnout: A Meta-Analysis of Longitudinal Studies. Int J Environ Res Public Health (2022) 19(20):13037. doi:10.3390/ijerph192013037
19. Brewer, EW, and Shapard, L. Employee Burnout: A Meta-Analysis of the Relationship Between Age or Years of Experience. Hum Resource Dev Rev (2004) 3(2):102–23. doi:10.1177/1534484304263335
20. Ong, J, Swift, C, Bath, M, Ong, S, Lim, W, Al-Naeeb, Y, et al. The Prevalence of Burnout, Risk Factors, and Job-Related Stressors in Gastroenterologists: A Systematic Review. J Gastroenterol Hepatol (2021) 36(9):2338–48. doi:10.1111/jgh.15488
21. Purvanova, RK, and Muros, JP. Gender Differences in Burnout: A Meta-Analysis. J Vocational Behav (2010) 77(2):168–85. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2010.04.006
22. Aronsson, G, Theorell, T, Grape, T, Hammarström, A, Hogstedt, C, Marteinsdottir, I, et al. A Systematic Review Including Meta-Analysis of Work Environment and Burnout Symptoms. BMC Public Health (2017) 17(1):264. doi:10.1186/s12889-017-4153-7
23. Koutsimani, P, Montgomery, A, and Georganta, K. The Relationship Between Burnout, Depression, and Anxiety: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Front Psychol (2019) 10:284. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00284
24. Ahola, K, Honkonen, T, Pirkola, S, Isometsä, E, Kalimo, R, Nykyri, E, et al. Alcohol Dependence in Relation to Burnout Among the Finnish Working Population. Addiction (2006) 101(10):1438–43. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01539.x
25. Velando-Soriano, A, Ortega-Campos, E, Gómez-Urquiza, JL, Ramírez-Baena, L, De La Fuente, EI, and Cañadas-De La Fuente, GA. Impact of Social Support in Preventing Burnout Syndrome in Nurses: A Systematic Review. Jpn J Nurs Sci (2020) 17(1):e12269. doi:10.1111/jjns.12269
26. Lau, B. Effort-Reward Imbalance and Overcommitment in Employees in a Norwegian Municipality: A Cross Sectional Study. J Occup Med Toxicol (2008) 3:9. doi:10.1186/1745-6673-3-9
27. Wege, N, Li, J, and Siegrist, J. Are There Gender Differences in Associations of Effort-Reward Imbalance at Work With Self-Reported Doctor-Diagnosed Depression? Prospective Evidence From the German Socio-Economic Panel. Int Arch Occup Environ Health (2018) 91(4):435–43. doi:10.1007/s00420-018-1293-8
28. Anne, K, Mika, K, Marianna, V, Jaana, P, and Jussi, V. Work Stress, Smoking Status, and Smoking Intensity: An Observational Study of 46 190 Employees. J Epidemiol Community Health (2005) 59(1):63–9. doi:10.1136/jech.2004.019752
29. Kouvonen, A, Kivimäki, M, Elovainio, M, Pentti, J, Linna, A, Virtanen, M, et al. Effort/Reward Imbalance and Sedentary Lifestyle: An Observational Study in a Large Occupational Cohort. Occup Environ Med (2006) 63(6):422–7. doi:10.1136/oem.2005.020974
30. Skogen, JC, Thørrisen, MM, Bonsaksen, T, Vahtera, J, Sivertsen, B, and Aas, RW. Effort-Reward Imbalance Is Associated With Alcohol-Related Problems. WIRUS-Screening Study. Front Psychol (2019) 10:2079. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02079
31. Rugulies, R, Aust, B, and Madsen, IEH. Effort-Reward Imbalance and Affective Disorders. Work Stress and Health in a Globalized Economy: The Model of Effort-Reward Imbalance. In: Aligning Perspectives on Health, Safety and Well-Being. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing/Springer Nature (2016). p. 103–43.
32. Rugulies, R, Aust, B, and Madsen, IE. Effort–Reward Imbalance at Work and Risk of Depressive Disorders. A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Prospective Cohort Studies. Scand J work, Environ Health (2017) 43(4):294–306. doi:10.5271/sjweh.3632
33. Vearing, A, and Mak, AS. Big Five Personality and Effort–Reward Imbalance Factors in Employees’ Depressive Symptoms. Personal Individual Differences (2007) 43(7):1744–55. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.05.011
34. Törnroos, M, Hintsanen, M, Hintsa, T, Jokela, M, Pulkki-Råback, L, Kivimäki, M, et al. Personality Traits of the Five-Factor Model Are Associated With Effort-Reward Imbalance at Work: A Population-Based Study. J Occup Environ Med (2012) 54(7):875–80. doi:10.1097/JOM.0b013e31824fe0e4
35. Hadžibajramović, E, Ahlborg, G, and Grimby-Ekman, A Concurrent and Lagged Effects of Psychosocial Job Stressors on Symptoms of Burnout. Int Arch Occup Environ Health (2019) 92(7):1013–21. doi:10.1007/s00420-019-01437-0
36. Leineweber, C, Bernhard-Oettel, C, Eib, C, Peristera, P, and Li, J. The Mediating Effect of Exhaustion in the Relationship Between Effort-Reward Imbalance and Turnover Intentions: A 4-Year Longitudinal Study From Sweden. J Occup Health (2021) 63(1):e12203. doi:10.1002/1348-9585.12203
37. Nuebling, M, Hegewald, J, Starke, KR, Lincke, HJ, Jankowiak, S, Liebers, F, et al. The Gutenberg Health Study: A Five-Year Prospective Analysis of Psychosocial Working Conditions Using COPSOQ (Copenhagen Psychosocial Questionnaire) and ERI (Effort-Reward Imbalance). BMC Public Health (2022) 22(1):24. doi:10.1186/s12889-021-12240-3
38. Firmann, M, Mayor, V, Vidal, PM, Bochud, M, Pécoud, A, Hayoz, D, et al. The CoLaus Study: A Population-Based Study to Investigate the Epidemiology and Genetic Determinants of Cardiovascular Risk Factors and Metabolic Syndrome. BMC Cardiovasc Disord (2008) 8:6. doi:10.1186/1471-2261-8-6
39. Preisig, M, Waeber, G, Vollenweider, P, Bovet, P, Rothen, S, Vandeleur, C, et al. The PsyCoLaus Study: Methodology and Characteristics of the Sample of a Population-Based Survey on Psychiatric Disorders and Their Association With Genetic and Cardiovascular Risk Factors. BMC Psychiatry (2009) 9(1):9. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-9-9
40. Siegrist, J. Chapter 9 - Effort-Reward Imbalance Model. In: G Fink, editor. Stress: Concepts, Cognition, Emotion, and Behavior. San Diego: Academic Press (2016). p. 81–6.
41. Niedhammer, I, Siegrist, J, Landre, MF, Goldberg, M, and Leclerc, A. Psychometric Properties of the French Version of the Effort-Reward Imbalance Model. Revue d'Epidemiologie et de Sante Publique (2000) 48(5):419–37.
42. Siegrist, J, Li, J, and Montano, M. Psychometric Properties of the Effort–Reward Imbalance Questionnaire (2014). Available From: http://www.uniklinik-duesseldorf.de/fileadmin/Datenpool/einrichtungen/institut_fuer_medizinische_soziologie_id54/ERI/PsychometricProperties.pdf (Accessed April 21, 2023).
43. Bocerean, C, Dupret, E, and Feltrin, M. Maslach Burnout Inventory -General Survey: French Validation in a Representative Sample of Employees. SCIREA J Health (2019) 3(2):24–41.
44. Schaufeli, W, Leiter, M, Maslach, C, and Jackson, S. The MBI–General Survey. In: C Maslach, S Jackson, and M Leiter, editors. Maslach Burnout Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press (1996).
45. Leboyer, M, Barbe, B, Gorwood, P, Teherani, M, Allilaire, J, Preisig, M, et al. Interview Diagnostique Pour les Etudes Génétiques. Paris: INSERM (1995).
46. Nurnberger, JI, Blehar, MC, Kaufmann, CA, York-Cooler, C, Simpson, SG, Harkavy-Friedman, J, et al. Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies: Rationale, Unique Features, and Training. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1994) 51(11):849–59. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950110009002
47. Preisig, M, Fenton, BT, Matthey, M-L, Berney, A, and Ferrero, F. Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies (DIGS): Inter-Rater and Test-Retest Reliability of the French Version. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (1999) 249(4):174–9. doi:10.1007/s004060050084
48. Berney, A, Preisig, M, Matthey, M-L, Ferrero, F, and Fenton, BT. Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies (DIGS): Inter-Rater and Test-Retest Reliability of Alcohol and Drug Diagnoses. Drug and Alcohol Dependence (2002) 65(2):149–58. doi:10.1016/s0376-8716(01)00156-9
49. Leboyer, M, Maier, W, Teherani, M, Lichtermann, D, D'Amato, T, Franke, P, et al. The Reliability of the SADS-LA in a Family Study Setting. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (1991) 241(3):165–9. doi:10.1007/BF02219716
50. Endicott, J, and Spitzer, RL. A Diagnostic Interview: The Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1978) 35(7):837–44. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1978.01770310043002
51. Association, AP. Task Force on DSM-IV. DSM-IV-TR: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC, USA: American Psychiatric Association (2000). XXXVII, 943.
52. Eysenck, H, Eysenck, S, Gauquelin, M, Gauquelin, F, Pascal, J, and Pascal, D. La Structure de la Personnalité Chez des Français Confrontée à Celle des Anglais Comparaison "Cross-Culturelle". Personnalité (1980) 1980:7–29.
53. Eysenck, HJ, and Eysenck, SBG. Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. London: Hodder and Stoughton (1975).
54. Denis, A, Callahan, S, and Bouvard, M. Evaluation of the French Version of the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support During the Postpartum Period. Matern Child Health J (2015) 19(6):1245–51. doi:10.1007/s10995-014-1630-9
55. Zimet, GD, Dahlem, NW, Zimet, SG, and Farley, GK. The Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support. J Personal Assess (1988) 52(1):30–41. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa5201_2
56. Dormann, CF, Elith, J, Bacher, S, Buchmann, C, Carl, G, Carré, G, et al. Collinearity: A Review of Methods to Deal With It and a Simulation Study Evaluating Their Performance. Ecography (2013) 36(1):27–46. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0587.2012.07348.x
57. Jakobsen, JC, Gluud, C, Wetterslev, J, and Winkel, P. When and How Should Multiple Imputation Be Used for Handling Missing Data in Randomised Clinical Trials – A Practical Guide With Flowcharts. BMC Med Res Methodol (2017) 17(1):162. doi:10.1186/s12874-017-0442-1
58. Schafer, JL. Multiple Imputation: A Primer. Stat Methods Med Res (1999) 8(1):3–15. doi:10.1177/096228029900800102
60. Leiter, MP, and Maslach, C. The Impact of Interpersonal Environment on Burnout and Organizational Commitment. J Organizational Behav (1988) 9(4):297–308. doi:10.1002/job.4030090402
61. Shirom, A, and Melamed, S. A Comparison of the Construct Validity of Two Burnout Measures in Two Groups of Professionals. Int J Stress Manage (2006) 13(2):176–200. doi:10.1037/1072-5245.13.2.176
62. Kristensen, TS, Borritz, M, Villadsen, E, and Christensen, KB. The Copenhagen Burnout Inventory: A New Tool for the Assessment of Burnout. Work & Stress (2005) 19(3):192–207. doi:10.1080/02678370500297720
63. Bianchi, R Burnout Is More Strongly Linked to Neuroticism Than to Work-Contextualized Factors. Psychiatry Res (2018) 270:901–5. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2018.11.015
64. Bianchi, R, Mayor, E, Schonfeld, IS, and Laurent, E. Burnout and Depressive Symptoms Are Not Primarily Linked to Perceived Organizational Problems. Psychol Health Med (2018) 23(9):1094–105. doi:10.1080/13548506.2018.1476725
65. Gerber, M, Schilling, R, Colledge, F, Ludyga, S, Pühse, U, and Brand, S. More Than a Simple Pastime? The Potential of Physical Activity to Moderate the Relationship Between Occupational Stress and Burnout Symptoms. Int J Stress Manage (2020) 27(1):53–64. doi:10.1037/str0000129
66. Hämmig, O. Explaining Burnout and the Intention to Leave the Profession Among Health Professionals – A Cross-Sectional Study in a Hospital Setting in Switzerland. BMC Health Serv Res (2018) 18(1):785. doi:10.1186/s12913-018-3556-1
67. Hämmig, O, Brauchli, R, and Bauer, GF. Effort-Reward and Work-Life Imbalance, General Stress and Burnout Among Employees of a Large Public Hospital in Switzerland. Swiss Med weekly (2012) 142:w13577. doi:10.4414/smw.2012.13577
68. Hausler, N, Bopp, M, and Hammig, O. Effort-Reward Imbalance, Work-Privacy Conflict, and Burnout Among Hospital Employees. J Occup Environ Med (2018) 60(4):E183–7. doi:10.1097/JOM.0000000000001287
69. Schilling, R, Colledge, F, Ludyga, S, Pühse, U, Brand, S, and Gerber, M. Does Cardiorespiratory Fitness Moderate the Association Between Occupational Stress, Cardiovascular Risk, and Mental Health in Police Officers? Int J Environ Res Public Health (2019) 16(13):2349. doi:10.3390/ijerph16132349
70. Avanzi, L, Zaniboni, S, Balducci, C, and Fraccaroli, F. The Relation Between Overcommitment and Burnout: Does It Depend on Employee Job Satisfaction? Anxiety, Stress, & Coping (2014) 27(4):455–65. doi:10.1080/10615806.2013.866230
71. Ding, Y, Qu, J, Yu, X, and Wang, S. The Mediating Effects of Burnout on the Relationship Between Anxiety Symptoms and Occupational Stress Among Community Healthcare Workers in China: A Cross-Sectional Study. PLoS One (2014) 9(9):e107130. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0107130
72. Wang, Z, Liu, H, Yu, H, Wu, Y, Chang, S, and Wang, L. Associations Between Occupational Stress, Burnout and Well-Being Among Manufacturing Workers: Mediating Roles of Psychological Capital and Self-Esteem. BMC Psychiatry (2017) 17(1):364. doi:10.1186/s12888-017-1533-6
73. Office Fédéral de la Statistique. L'enquête Suisse sur la Santé 2017 en Bref Conception, Méthode, Réalisation. Switzerland: Office fédéral de la statistique (2018). OFS2018.
74. Naczenski, LM, Vries, JD, Hooff, M, and Kompier, MAJ. Systematic Review of the Association Between Physical Activity and Burnout. J Occup Health (2017) 59(6):477–94. doi:10.1539/joh.17-0050-RA
75. Vandeleur, CL, Fassassi, S, Castelao, E, Glaus, J, Strippoli, MF, Lasserre, AM, et al. Prevalence and Correlates of DSM-5 Major Depressive and Related Disorders in the Community. Psychiatry Res (2017) 250:50–8. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2017.01.060
76. Watson, D, and Pennebaker, JW. Health Complaints, Stress, and Distress: Exploring the Central Role of Negative Affectivity. Psychol Rev (1989) 96(2):234–54. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.96.2.234
77. Caputo, A Social Desirability Bias in Self-Reported Well-Being Measures: Evidence From an Online Survey. Universitas Psychologica (2017) 16:245–55. doi:10.11144/javeriana.upsy16-2.sdsw
79. Hertel, G, Rauschenbach, C, Thielgen, MM, and Krumm, S. Are Older Workers More Active Copers? Longitudinal Effects of Age-Contingent Coping on Strain at Work. J Organizational Behav (2015) 36(4):514–37. doi:10.1002/job.1995
Keywords: prospective cohort, occupational health, burnout, general population, exposure to work-related stress
Citation: Shoman Y, Ranjbar S, Strippoli M-P, von Känel R, Preisig M and Guseva Canu I (2023) Relationship Between Effort-Reward Imbalance, Over-Commitment and Occupational Burnout in the General Population: A Prospective Cohort Study. Int J Public Health 68:1606160. doi: 10.3389/ijph.2023.1606160
Received: 04 May 2023; Accepted: 21 September 2023;
Published: 06 October 2023.
Edited by:
Taulant Muka, Epistudia, SwitzerlandReviewed by:
Elena Lisá, Comenius University, SlovakiaHelga Dizdari, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois (CHUV), Switzerland
Copyright © 2023 Shoman, Ranjbar, Strippoli, von Känel, Preisig and Guseva Canu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Yara Shoman, yara.shoman@unil.ch