- 1Department of Psychology, Nanjing University, Nanjing, China
- 2Department of Psychology, Shanghai Normal University, Shanghai, China
- 3Department of Psychology, Faculty of Social Sciences, University of Macau, Macau, China
- 4Department of Psychology, Suzhou University of Science and Technology, Suzhou, China
- 5Institute of Education, Nanjing University, Nanjing, China
- 6State Key Laboratory of Media Convergence and Communication, Communication University of China, Beijing, China
Objectives: Previous studies revealed a positive association between neuroticism and depression. This study further extended the previous findings by exploring the psychological processes underlying this association among Chinese postgraduates. Guided by theoretical models and empirical research, we proposed a multiple mediation and moderated mediation model to investigate the roles of dispositional mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal in the relationship between neuroticism and depression.
Methods: Using the NEO Five-Factor Inventory, Beck Depression Inventory, Mindfulness Attention Awareness Scale, and Emotion Regulation Questionnaire, 1103 first-year postgraduates at a comprehensive university in China were surveyed. Path analysis was adopted to test the models.
Results: The results showed that dispositional mindfulness mediated the association between neuroticism and depression. Further, this mediating effect was moderated by cognitive reappraisal, with this effect being stronger in individuals with low engagement in cognitive reappraisal.
Conclusion: The results support interrelations among neuroticism, depression, dispositional mindfulness, and cognitive reappraisal as moderated mediation rather than multiple mediation. The results enhance our understanding of psychological mechanisms between neuroticism and depression and provide suggestions for interventions to prevent or reduce depression in highly neurotic postgraduates.
Introduction
On 23 February 2017, the World Health Organization reported that 322 million people suffer from depression worldwide. Among them, young people, pregnant and postpartum women, and elderly are especially vulnerable to depression [1]. Evans et al. (2018) evaluated 2279 postgraduates who completed clinically validated scales for anxiety and depression via social media and email, and reported that 41% of postgraduates manifested moderate-to-severe anxiety and 39% of postgraduates exhibited moderate-to-severe depression [2]. Similar mental health issues were prevalent in China before [3] and during [4] the Coronavirus disease (COVID-19) pandemic. Gewin (2012) stated that depression was prevalent among postgraduate and postdoctoral students, who experienced pressure from academic studies, interpersonal relationships, and career planning, even in the first year of study [5, 6]. Accordingly, it is crucial to investigate and prevent depression in postgraduates.
The five-factor model of personality is one of the most influential personality theories [7]. The five factors, labeled extraversion, neuroticism, openness, agreeableness and conscientiousness, were identified in self-rating and peer rating scales [8, 9], and in populations with personality disorder [10]. Among the five factors, neuroticism indicates emotional instability and predicts negative outcomes in daily life, and is a powerful predictor of depression, anxiety, and other emotional disorders [11–13]. Although extant findings suggest the compelling role of neuroticism in the etiology and maintenance of depression, little is known about these associations among postgraduate students, and the factors underlying this relationship are yet to be explored. Therefore, this study further extended previous studies by exploring the psychological processes underlying the association between neuroticism and depression and examining the roles of dispositional mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal among Chinese postgraduates.
Mediation Role of Dispositional Mindfulness
Mindfulness is defined as the state of being attentive to and aware of current situations [14]. Accordingly, dispositional mindfulness refers to an individual’s ability to be attentive to and aware of the present situation, i.e., self-awareness [15]. However, individuals with severe neuroticism fear of potential future threats (i.e., worry) and reflect more on previous negative emotional experiences (i.e., rumination) than individuals with less severe neuroticism [16, 17]. Thus, individuals with severe neuroticism face difficulty in concentrating their attention and consciousness on their present feelings in addition to problems entering a state of mindfulness [18, 19]. Accordingly, their levels of dispositional mindfulness tend to be low.
Previous studies also revealed negative relationships between dispositional mindfulness and depression in specific populations and occupations [20, 21] as well as general population during COVID-19 pandemic [22, 23]. This may be because attentiveness and awareness of the present situation help individuals disengage from previous negative emotional experiences, as claimed by Desrosiers et al. (2013) [24]. In addition, Giluk (2009) conducted a meta-analysis and revealed strong negative correlations between dispositional mindfulness and neuroticism (r = −0.45) and between dispositional mindfulness and trait negative affect (r = −0.39) [25]. Thus, we hypothesized that dispositional mindfulness mediates the relationship between neuroticism and depression.
It should be noted that although neuroticism and dispositional mindfulness are both trait types, they are distinct at the hierarchical level from the perspective of core versus surface characteristics [26]. Neuroticism is considered a core characteristic, a pattern of thoughts, feelings, and actions that is not susceptible to change over time or by the situation. In contrast, dispositional mindfulness is considered a surface characteristic, that is, a trait that emerges later and is less stable than the core traits and can be cultivated and strengthened through various interventions, such as training programs [27]. Empirical research evidence seems to be consistent with this view. Neuroticism is associated with being more genetically based, whereas dispositional mindfulness has been found to be more influenced by the environment, such as attachment styles, child abuse, organizational climate of care, and trauma exposure [28–32]. This is also supported by evidence of heritability, which ranges from 40% to 60% for neuroticism [33], but only 32% for dispositional mindfulness [34]. Thus, it appears that dispositional mindfulness is a less stable and more environmentally susceptible surface manifestation of personality. Thus, the relationship between neuroticism and dispositional mindfulness reflects the interaction of different levels of traits, meaning that dispositional mindfulness is similar to the interaction between neuroticism and environmental factors. This may also be the reason why many researchers are interested in the relationship between personality factors and dispositional mindfulness [25, 35–37].
Role of Cognitive Reappraisal
Cognitive reappraisal refers to thinking about events in an objective, neutral, and positive manner [38]. A greater tendency to engage in cognitive reappraisal is associated with fewer symptoms of depression [39]. Further, high levels of neuroticism are associated with a lower tendency to engage in cognitive reappraisal [40]. Therefore, individuals with less severe neuroticism may be less likely to experience depression because they tend to engage in cognitive reappraisal and thus more effectively regulate negative emotion.
Further, focusing on the present may expand the awareness of various stimuli, so as to identify negative cognition and emotions more accurately and promptly [41]. Previous studies suggested that high dispositional mindfulness was associated with high consistency between implicit and explicit measures of emotion [14], which indicated that individuals with high dispositional mindfulness are more aware of their feelings. In addition, enhanced awareness increases the information available to individuals, which facilitates successful reappraisal of the current situation. Empirical studies found that mindfulness training [42] especially state mindfulness during meditation [43] was associated with an increased tendency to engage in cognitive reappraisal. Accordingly, we hypothesized that cognitive reappraisal mediates the relationships between neuroticism and depression and between dispositional mindfulness and depression.
However, developing reappraisal skills is not a specific goal of mindfulness practices [24]. Thus, the association between dispositional mindfulness and depression possibly depends on the frequency of engagement in cognitive reappraisal. Although most previous studies have assessed the role of cognitive reappraisal as a mediator in the relationship between dispositional mindfulness and outcomes [44, 45], it is also reasonable to assume that individuals with high levels of dispositional mindfulness may be less susceptible to depression for those able to use cognitive reappraisal easily than those who use cognitive reappraisal hard and the ease of using cognitive reappraisal depends on the recency and frequency of strategies used in similar emotional situations [46]. Accordingly, cognitive reappraisal plays a moderated mediation role in the relationship between neuroticism and depression.
Current Study: Testing Moderated Mediation and Multiple Mediation Models
This study first assessed whether dispositional mindfulness mediated the relationship between neuroticism and depression among first-year postgraduates in China. Further, this study examined the role of cognitive reappraisal in the relationship between neuroticism and depression. Based on a literature review, this study proposed the following three hypotheses. Support for H1 and H2 suggests a multiple mediation relationship (see Figure 1A), whereas support for H1 and H3 suggests a moderated mediation relationship (see Figure 1B).
FIGURE 1. Conceptual model of the effect of neuroticism on depression (China, 2017). (A) The multiple mediation model. (B) The moderated mediation model.
H1. Dispositional mindfulness mediates the relationship between neuroticism and depression.
H2. Cognitive reappraisal mediates the relationship between neuroticism and depression. In addition, neuroticism negatively predicts dispositional mindfulness, which predicts more frequent cognitive reappraisal and thereby lower levels of depression.
H3. Cognitive reappraisal moderates the mediating effect of dispositional mindfulness on the relationship between neuroticism and depression. This mediating effect is stronger in individuals with low engagement in cognitive reappraisal.
Methods
Participants
We used convenience sampling to recruit 1200 newly enrolled postgraduates at a comprehensive university in China. We excluded invalid responses with incomplete answers (constituting at least 20% of the unanswered questions, and at least one blank questionnaire), inconsistent answers (with clear contradiction), or obvious regularities (e.g., choosing one answer for at least 10 consecutive questions). Accordingly, 1103 students (55.30% female) completed the survey with an effective response rate of 91.92%. The average age of the participants was 24.32 ± 2.92 years, ranging from 18 to 43 years. Among them, 198 were general doctoral students (including those admitted through general admission and the application exam); 60 were direct postgraduates (equivalent to first-year master’s students); 456 were academic master’s students; 351 were professional master’s students; and 32 were business administration master’s students. The degree programs of 6 participants were unspecified.
This study was approved via the Ethical Evaluation of Research Projects at the Department of Psychology in the School for Social and Behavioral Sciences at Nanjing University. All procedures involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional or national research committee and with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all participants included in the study.
Instruments
NEO Five-Factor Inventory
The sub-scale of Chinese version of the NFFI was employed to measure participants’ levels of neuroticism [47, 48]. Participants rated 12 items (e.g., “I often feel nervous and uneasy”) from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The scale has an acceptable Cronbach’s alpha score of 0.87.
Beck Depression Inventory
The Chinese version of the BDI was employed to measure participants’ depression levels during the preceding week [49, 50]. Participants rated 21 items (e.g., “I am not a loser”) from 0 (no) to 3 (absolutely). The scale has an acceptable Cronbach’s alpha score of 0.83.
Mindfulness Attention Awareness Scale
The Chinese version of the MAAS was employed to measure participants’ dispositional mindfulness [14, 51]. Participants rated 15 items (e.g., “I could be experiencing an emotion and not be conscious of it until sometime later”) on a scale of 1 (almost always) to 6 (almost never). The scale has an acceptable Cronbach’s alpha score of 0.87.
Emotion Regulation Questionnaire
The Chinese version of the subdimension of cognitive reappraisal of the ERQ was employed to measure participants’ engagement of cognitive reappraisal [52, 53]. Participants rated 6 items (e.g., “When I want to feel fewer negative emotions, I change the way I am thinking about the situation”) on a scale of 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The scale has an acceptable Cronbach’s alpha score of 0.81.
Data Analysis
We used SPSS 22.0 to conduct descriptive statistics and perform correlation analysis. Subsequently, we used the SPSS PROCESS macro to analyze the multiple mediation model (Model 6) and moderated mediation model (Model 14) [54]. Notably, age, gender, and student types were considered covariates in the multiple mediation and moderated mediation models. In addition, the difference between the results of unadjusted and adjusted analysis was minor. The unadjusted analysis results are reported in the Supplementary Materials.
Results
Preliminary Analysis
Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics and Pearson correlations of all variables. Neuroticism was positively correlated with depression and negatively correlated with dispositional mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal. Depression was negatively correlated with dispositional mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal. Dispositional mindfulness was positively correlated with cognitive reappraisal.
The average BDI score of the surveyed first-year postgraduates indicated mild depression. The median score (4.0) indicated minimal depression. In addition, a positive rate of depression was calculated. Among all the first-year postgraduates, 596 (54.03%) manifested minimal depressive symptoms (score ≤4), 375 (34.00%) had mild depression (5–13), 99 (8.98%) had moderate depression (14–20), and 33 (2.99%) had severe depression (≥21).
Testing the Multiple Mediation Model
The main results revealed that after the inclusion of covariates, neuroticism positively predicted depression (β = 0.58, t = 21.48, p < 0.001) and negatively predicted dispositional mindfulness (β = − 0.51, t = −19.29, p < 0.001) and cognitive reappraisal (β = − 0.18, t = −5.45, p < 0.001). Further, dispositional mindfulness negatively predicted depression (β = − 0.13, t = −4.80, p < 0.001) and positively predicted cognitive reappraisal (β = 0.26, t = 7.91, p < 0.001). However, the effect of cognitive reappraisal on depression was not significant (β = − 0.02, t = −0.86, p = 0.39). Accordingly, the multiple mediation model was not supported because of the nonsignificant effect of cognitive reappraisal on depression.
Testing the Moderated Mediation Model
Table 2 and Figure 2 presented the main results generated by the SPSS PROCESS macro (Model 14) [54]. The model comprised a (1) mediator and dependent variable model and (2) conditional indirect effect. The mediator and dependent variable model revealed that after the inclusion of covariates, neuroticism positively predicted depression (β = 0.58, t = 21.53, p < 0.001) and negatively predicted dispositional mindfulness (β = − 0.51, t = −19.29, p < 0.001). Further, dispositional mindfulness negatively predicted depression (β = − 0.14, t = −5.07, p < 0.001). A bootstrap procedure was conducted to assess the size of the indirect effect and obtain confidence intervals (CIs). Using random sampling, we generated 1000 bootstrapping samples from the original dataset. The indirect effect of neuroticism on depression through dispositional mindfulness was 0.07 (95% CI = [0.04, 0.10]). Because the 95% CI did not include 0, dispositional mindfulness was considered to mediate the effect of neuroticism on depression.
TABLE 2. The moderated mediation effect of dispositional mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal on neuroticism and depression (China, 2017).
FIGURE 2. The moderated mediation model of the relationship between neuroticism, dispositional mindfulness, cognitive reappraisal, and depression (China, 2017).
The interaction between dispositional mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal positively predicted depression (β = 0.06, t = 2.70, p = 0.007). These results indicate that cognitive reappraisal moderates the mediating effect of dispositional mindfulness on the relationship between neuroticism and depression.
A simple slope test was conducted to further elucidate the moderating effect of cognitive reappraisal. The simple slope test was used to examine the differences in the effect of dispositional mindfulness on depression between individuals who frequently engaged in cognitive reappraisal and those who did not (Figure 3) [55]. For individuals with low cognitive reappraisal scores (mean – 1SD), dispositional mindfulness was significantly negatively associated with depression (β = − 0.45, t = −9.66, p < 0.001). For individuals with high cognitive reappraisal scores (mean + 1SD), this association was significant but weaker (β = − 0.34, t = −7.86, p < 0.001).
FIGURE 3. Effect of the interaction between dispositional mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal on depression (China, 2017).
Discussion
This study analyzed the mediating effect of dispositional mindfulness on the relationship between neuroticism and depression. Further, the study determined whether cognitive reappraisal mediates or moderates the aforementioned effect. To our knowledge, this study was the first to investigate the relationship among neuroticism, dispositional mindfulness, cognitive reappraisal, and depression among Chinese postgraduates. The integrated model contributes to our understanding of which reactivity processes link neuroticism and depression and whether these processes differ among individuals. The integrated model also provides empirical evidence of the role of cognitive reappraisal in the relationship among neuroticism, dispositional mindfulness, and depression.
The preliminary analysis revealed that the average BDI scores of the Chinese first-year postgraduates indicated mild depression. Further, nearly half of the participants experienced depression to varying degrees. Although the positive rate differed from the incidence of clinical depression, the findings were consistent with those of an earlier study, which revealed a positive SCL-90 rate of 31.81% among Chinese first-year graduate students [56].
The correlation analysis revealed that neuroticism was positively correlated with depression, which was consistent with those of previous studies involving other age groups [57, 58]. Environment and personality influence emotional response. Although extroversion and neuroticism are both personality traits with typical emotional styles [59], they affect emotional response differently. Specifically, extroversion is related to positive emotional response, whereas neuroticism is related to negative emotional response [60]. When something positive occurs, extroversion triggers strong emotions; a negative event also results in strong emotional behavior, but the emotional state is restored quickly. Neuroticism elicits stronger physiological response and emotional feelings in negative situations [59].
The results of this study elucidate the mediating role of dispositional mindfulness in the relationship between neuroticism and depression. Previous studies have suggested that dispositional mindfulness is correlated with neuroticism and negative emotions [25] and mediated the relationship between neuroticism and subjective well-being or impulsiveness [61]. This study extended previous findings by investigating the role of dispositional mindfulness in mediating the relationship between neuroticism and depression. The results indicate that individuals with severe neuroticism have low dispositional mindfulness, and high depression tendency.
Based on previous results, our study proposed a multiple mediation model including neuroticism, dispositional mindfulness, cognitive reappraisal and depression. Our study suggests that the multiple mediation model was not supported because of the nonsignificant effect of cognitive reappraisal on depression. Notably, the analysis of multiple mediation effects of dispositional mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal revealed significant effects of mediation depending on the strength of the association. The correlation analysis revealed that dispositional mindfulness (−0.51 and −0.43) was more strongly correlated with neuroticism and depression than cognitive reappraisal (−0.29 and −0.24), which may lead to a masking effect as reported previously [62]. Our results indicate that dispositional mindfulness is more strongly associated with psychopathology than cognitive reappraisal. Further, the mediation effect of cognitive reappraisal on the relationship between dispositional mindfulness and depression was not supported, which was inconsistent with the results of previous studies [24]. This inconsistency stems mainly from the role of dispositional mindfulness in the model. For instance, Desrosiers et al. (2013) treated dispositional mindfulness as an independent variable rather than as a mediator as in this study [24].
In addition to the multiple mediation model, our results supported a moderated mediation model considering that cognitive reappraisal skill is not the specific goal of mindfulness practice [24]. Frequency of engagement in cognitive reappraisal strategies influenced the effect of dispositional mindfulness on depression, which may support the orienting attention/action readiness (OAAR) framework used in the emotion regulation field [46]. According to the OAAR framework, the interaction between action readiness and orienting attention determines the outcomes of emotional regulation. Individuals trained in meditation exhibit high levels of dispositional mindfulness and can easily enter a state of orienting attention [63], while individual readiness to implement specific regulatory strategies (e.g., cognitive reappraisal) increases with the frequency of strategies used in similar emotional situations. The present study provides an empirical evidence-based support for the OAAR framework by using attention-oriented ability (i.e., dispositional mindfulness) as a mediating variable and adaptive emotion regulation strategies (i.e., cognitive reappraisal) as a moderating variable. The result indicates that individuals with low dispositional mindfulness have higher depressive tendency; however, the frequency of cognitive reappraisal played a buffering role in this relationship. It is important to note that regardless of the frequency of cognitive reappraisal, with the decrease of dispositional mindfulness, the level of depression increases, but the rate of increase varies with the frequency of using cognitive reappraisal. Thus, cognitive reappraisal only altered the strength of the relationship between dispositional mindfulness and depression, but not the relationship or its direction.
The limitations of this study are notable. First, the research questions were related to causality, but the cross-sectional design and mediation analysis could not indicate causation [64]. Future longitudinal studies are required to specify the directionality of the relationships among neuroticism, dispositional mindfulness, cognitive reappraisal, and depression. Second, we recruited only first-year postgraduates via convenience sampling; thus our data are not representative of all postgraduates. Third, although the interaction between dispositional mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal positively predicted depression, the effect size was relatively small. Caution is therefore essential when drawing conclusions. Fourth, this study explored the roles of dispositional mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal; however, the roles of other emotional regulation strategies (e.g., expressive suppression) remain unexplored. Finally, although neuroticism and dispositional mindfulness are two levels of traits, exploring the relationship between the two traits using subjective scales is less clinically valuable than exploring the relationship between neuroticism and mindfulness using mindfulness-based training. Previous studies found that personality factors (especially the neuroticism dimension) may contribute to the outcome of mindfulness training. For example, de Vibe et al. (2015) found a greater effect of the mindfulness-based intervention on mental distress in highly neurotic students than in lowly neurotic students [65]. Nyklíček1 and Irrmischer (2017) found that highly neurotic participants showed stronger decreases in both anxious and depressed mood than lowly neurotic students [66]. Jagielski et al. (2019) found that the individuals with high levels of neuroticism manifested significantly lower levels of distress at 12-month follow-up compared with those who with low levels of neuroticism [67]. Above all, individuals with high neuroticism tended to be the greatest beneficiaries of mindfulness training. Therefore, we should examine whether baseline neuroticism levels can predict changes in mindfulness and changes in depressive symptoms during mindfulness training to further explore the relationship among neuroticism, mindfulness, and depression in the future.
Despite the aforementioned limitations, this study has key theoretical and practical implications. From a theoretical perspective, this study extends previous research and improves the understanding of how and when individuals with severe neuroticism develop depression. Next, we theoretically clarified the relationship among neuroticism, dispositional mindfulness, cognitive reappraisal and depression using multiple and moderated mediation models with the same sample and variables. Finally, the results provide integrated evidence supporting the OAAR framework. From a practical perspective, our findings are critical for interventions aimed at preventing or reducing depression. Neuroticism has a significant predictive effect on depression. Accordingly, we recommend increased emphasis on mental health education of individuals with severe neuroticism. Further, neuroticism predicted depression not only directly but also through mediation via dispositional mindfulness. Accordingly, individuals with severe neuroticism can be assisted via mindfulness-based interventions. In addition, cognitive reappraisal played a moderating role in the relation between dispositional mindfulness and depression. Thus, interventions combining cognitive reappraisal and mindfulness (e.g., Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy or MBCT) were more effective than mindfulness solely (e.g., Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction or MBSR) for individuals with severe neuroticism to reduce depression. Querstret et al. (2020) reported a meta-analysis indicating that MBCT generated larger effect sizes than MBSR for depression, suggesting the possibility that the cognitive component of MBCT may contribute to participants’ mindfulness learning, thereby increasing self-reported mindfulness skills [68]. The results of this study using a subjective questionnaire also demonstrated the superiority of MBCT, and therefore MBCT should be used in future clinical practice whenever possible. Of course, a comparative clinical intervention study should be conducted in the future to further validate the differences between the two approaches.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the body for ethical evaluation of research projects at the Department of Psychology—part of the School for Social and Behavioral Sciences at Nanjing University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
All authors have made a significant contribution to this work. LH, LRC, and JF collected the data. LH and RZ analyzed and interpreted the data. LH and LC wrote the current version of the manuscript. All authors have approved the final version of the article.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.ssph-journal.org/articles/10.3389/ijph.2022.1605074/full#supplementary-material
References
1.World Health Organization. Depression and Other Common Mental Disorders: Global Health Estimates. Geneva: World Health Organization (2017).
2. Evans, T, Bira, L, Gastelum, J, Weiss, L, and Vanderford, N. Evidence for a Mental Health Crisis in Graduate Education. Nat Biotechnol (2018) 36(3):282–4. doi:10.1038/nbt.4089
3. Guo, LP, Li, ZH, Chen, TL, Liu, GH, Fan, HY, and Yang, KH. The Prevalence of Mental Distress and Association with Social Changes Among Postgraduate Students in China: A Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis. Public Health (2020) 186:178–84. doi:10.1016/j.puhe.2020.06.014
4. Hou, L, Long, F, Meng, Y, Cheng, X, Zhang, W, and Zhou, R. The Relationship between Quarantine Length and Negative Affect during the COVID-19 Epidemic Among the General Population in China: The Roles of Negative Cognition and Protective Factors. Front Psychol (2021) 12:575684. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2021.575684
6. Zhang, DY, Zheng, K, and Liu, SS. Investigation and Research on Stressors of Graduate Students. Campus Life Ment Health (2018) 16(2):112–5. doi:10.19521/j.cnki.1673-1662.2018.02.007
7. McCrae, RR, and Costa, JPT. The Five-Factor Theory of Personality. In: John OP, Robins RW, and Pervin LA, editors. Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research. The Guilford Press (2008). p. 159–81.
8. Saucier, G. Effects of Variable Selection on the Factor Structure of Person Descriptors. J Pers Soc Psychol (1997) 73(6):1296–312. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.6.1296
9. Tupes, EC, and Christal, RE. Recurrent Personality Factors Based on Trait Ratings. J Pers (1992) 60(2):225–51. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00973.x
10. Clark, LA, and Livesley, WJ. Two Approaches to Identifying the Dimensions of Personality Disorder: Convergence on the Five-Factor Model. In: Costa JPT, and Wdiger TA, editors. Personality Disorders and the Five-Factor Model of Personality. American Psychological Association (2002). p. 161–76.
11. Barlow, DH, Curreri, AJ, and Woodard, LS. Neuroticism and Disorders of Emotion: A New Synthesis. Curr Dir Psychol Sci (2021) 30(5):410–7. doi:10.1177/09637214211030253
12. Chen, X, Pu, J, Shi, W, and Zhou, Y. The Impact of Neuroticism on Symptoms of Anxiety and Depression in Elderly Adults: The Mediating Role of Rumination. Curr Psychol (2020) 39(1):42–50. doi:10.1007/s12144-017-9740-3
13. Tonarely, NA, Sherman, JA, Grossman, RA, Shaw, AM, and Ehrenreich-May, J. Neuroticism as an Underlying Construct in Youth Emotional Disorders. Bull Menninger Clin (2020) 84(3):214–36. doi:10.1521/bumc.2020.84.3.214
14. Brown, KW, and Ryan, RM. The Benefits of Being Present: Mindfulness and its Role in Psychological Well-Being. J Pers Soc Psychol (2003) 84(4):822–48. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822
15. Xu, W, Liu, XH, and Liu, R. Effect of Mindfulness Training Improving Emotional Inertia: Moderation Role of Mindfulness Trait. Chin J Clin Psychol (2015) 23(6):1129–32. doi:10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2015.06.040
16. Watson, D, and Clark, LA. Negative Affectivity: The Disposition to Experience Aversive Emotional States. Psychol Bull (1984) 96(3):465–90. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.96.3.465
17. Denovan, A, Dagnall, N, and Lofthouse, G. Neuroticism and Somatic Complaints: Concomitant Effects of Rumination and Worry. Behav Cogn Psychother (2019) 47(4):431–45. doi:10.1017/S1352465818000619
18. Borkovec, TD, Ray, WJ, and Stober, J. Worry: A Cognitive Phenomenon Intimately Linked to Affective, Physiological, and Interpersonal Behavioral Processes. Cognit Ther Res (1998) 22(6):561–76. doi:10.1023/A:1018790003416
19. Matthews, G, and Wells, A. Attention and Emotion (Classic Edition): A Clinical Perspective. New York: Psychology Press (2014).
20. Di Giuseppe, M, Ciacchini, R, Piarulli, A, Nepa, G, and Conversano, C. Mindfulness Dispositions and Defense Style as Positive Responses to Psychological Distress in Oncology Professionals. Eur J Oncol Nurs (2019) 40:104–10. doi:10.1016/j.ejon.2019.04.003
21. Smith, BW, Ortiz, JA, Steffen, LE, Tooley, EM, Wiggins, KT, Yeater, EA, et al. Mindfulness Is Associated with Fewer PTSD Symptoms, Depressive Symptoms, Physical Symptoms, and Alcohol Problems in Urban Firefighters. J Consult Clin Psychol (2011) 79(5):613–7. doi:10.1037/a0025189
22. Conversano, C, Di Giuseppe, M, Miccoli, M, Ciacchini, R, Gemignani, A, and Orrù, G. Mindfulness, Age and Gender as Protective Factors against Psychological Distress during COVID-19 Pandemic. Front Psychol (2020) 11:1900. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01900
23. Sweeny, K, Rankin, K, Cheng, X, Hou, L, Long, F, Meng, Y, et al. Flow in the Time of COVID-19: Findings from China. PLoS One (2020) 15(11):e0242043. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0242043
24. Desrosiers, A, Vine, V, Klemanski, DH, and Nolen‐Hoeksema, S. Mindfulness and Emotion Regulation in Depression and Anxiety: Common and Distinct Mechanisms of Action. Depress Anxiety (2013) 30(7):654–61. doi:10.1002/da.22124
25. Giluk, TL. Mindfulness, Big Five Personality, and Affect: A Meta-Analysis. Pers Individ Dif (2009) 47(8):805–11. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.026
26. Kandler, C, Zimmermann, J, and McAdams, DP. Core and Surface Characteristics for the Description and Theory of Personality Differences and Development. Eur J Pers (2014) 28(3):231–43. doi:10.1002/per.1952
27. Ioannou, A, Tussyadiah, I, and Marshan, A. Dispositional Mindfulness as an Antecedent of Privacy Concerns: A protection Motivation Theory Perspective. Psychol Mark (2021) 38(10):1766–78. doi:10.1002/mar.21529
28. An, Y, Yuan, G, Zhang, N, Xu, W, Liu, Z, and Zhou, F. Longitudinal Cross-Lagged Relationships between Mindfulness, Posttraumatic Stress Symptoms, and Posttraumatic Growth in Adolescents Following the Yancheng Tornado in China. Psychiatry Res (2018) 266:334–40. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2018.03.034
29. Boughner, E, Thornley, E, Kharlas, D, and Frewen, P. Mindfulness-related Traits Partially Mediate the Association between Lifetime and Childhood Trauma Exposure and PTSD and Dissociative Symptoms in a Community Sample Assessed Online. Mindfulness (2016) 7(3):672–9. doi:10.1007/s12671-016-0502-3
30. Fitzgerald, M. Childhood Maltreatment is Associated with Adult Psychopathology through Decreased Dispositional Mindfulness. J Aggress Maltreat Trauma (Advance online Publication) (2022) 1–16. doi:10.1080/10926771.2022.2043971
31. Kalafatoğlu, Y, and Turgut, T. Individual and Organizational Antecedents of Trait Mindfulness. J Manag Spirituality Religion (2019) 16(2):199–220. doi:10.1080/14766086.2018.1541756
32. Pepping, CA, and Duvenage, M. The Origins of Individual Differences in Dispositional Mindfulness. Pers Individ Dif (2016) 93:130–6. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.05.027
33. Jang, KL, Livesley, WJ, and Vernon, PA. Heritability of the Big Five Personality Dimensions and Their Facets: A Twin Study. J Pers (1996) 64(3):577–91. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1996.tb00522.x
34. Waszczuk, MA, Zavos, HM, Antonova, E, Haworth, CM, Plomin, R, and Eley, TC. A Multivariate Twin Study of Trait Mindfulness, Depressive Symptoms, and Anxiety Sensitivity. Depress Anxiety (2015) 32(4):254–61. doi:10.1002/da.22326
35. Haliwa, I, Wilson, JM, Spears, SK, Strough, J, and Shook, NJ. Exploring Facets of the Mindful Personality: Dispositional Mindfulness and the Big Five. Pers Individ Dif (2021) 171:110469. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2020.110469
36. Hanley, AW. The Mindful Personality: Associations between Dispositional Mindfulness and the Five Factor Model of Personality. Pers Individ Dif (2016) 91:154–8. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.11.054
37. Hanley, AW, and Garland, EL. The Mindful Personality: A Meta-Analysis from a Cybernetic Perspective. Mindfulness (2017) 8(6):1456–70. doi:10.1007/s12671-017-0736-8
38. Gross, JJ. Emotion Regulation: Conceptual and Empirical Foundations. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of Emotion Regulation the Guilford Press (2014). p. 3–20.
39. Dryman, MT, and Heimberg, RG. Emotion Regulation in Social Anxiety and Depression: A Systematic Review of Expressive Suppression and Cognitive Reappraisal. Clin Psychol Rev (2018) 65:17–42. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2018.07.004
40. Zamani, E, Akbari, M, Mohammadkhani, S, Riskind, JH, Drake, CL, and Palagini, L. The Relationship of Neuroticism with Sleep Quality: The Mediating Role of Emotional, Cognitive and Metacognitive Factors. Behav Sleep Med (2022) 20(1):74–89. doi:10.1080/15402002.2021.1888730
41. Allen, NB, Blashki, G, and Gullone, E. Mindfulness-based Psychotherapies: A Review of Conceptual Foundations, Empirical Evidence and Practical Considerations. Aust N Z J Psychiatry (2006) 40(4):285–94. doi:10.1080/j.1440-1614.2006.01794.x
42. Garland, EL, Gaylord, SA, and Fredrickson, BL. Positive Reappraisal Mediates the Stress-Reductive Effects of Mindfulness: An Upward Spiral Process. Mindfulness (2011) 2(1):59–67. doi:10.1007/s12671-011-0043-8
43. Garland, EL, Hanley, A, Farb, NA, and Froeliger, B. State Mindfulness during Meditation Predicts Enhanced Cognitive Reappraisal. Mindfulness (2015) 6(2):234–42. doi:10.1007/s12671-013-0250-6
44. McKee, LG, Duprey, EB, and O’Neal, CW. Emotion Socialization and Young Adult Internalizing Symptoms: The Roles of Mindfulness and Emotion Regulation. Mindfulness (2021) 12(1):53–60. doi:10.1007/s12671-018-1079-9
45. Watford, TS, Braden, AL, and Emley, EA. Mediation of the Association between Mindfulness and Emotional Eating Among Overweight Individuals. Mindfulness (2019) 10(6):1153–62. doi:10.1007/s12671-018-1064-3
46. Ghafur, RD, Suri, G, and Gross, JJ. Emotion Regulation Choice: The Role of Orienting Attention and Action Readiness. Curr Opin Behav Sci (2018) 19:31–5. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.08.016
47. Costa, P, and McCrae, RR. NEO-PI-R Professional Manual: Revised NEO Personality and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI). In: Odessa FL, editor. Psychological Assessment Resources, 4 (1992).
48. Xi, C, Zhong, MT, Lei, XX, Liu, Y, Ling, Y, and Yi, JY. Comparing the Neuroticism Subscale of the NEO-PI-R with that of the NEO-FFI. Chin J Clin Psychol (2017) 25(3):453–6. doi:10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2017.03.012
50. Zhang, YX, Wang, Y, and Qian, MY. Reliability and Validity of Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) Examined in Chinese Samples. China J Health Psychol (1990) 4(4):164–8. doi:10.3321/j.issn:1000-6729.1990.04.005
51. Chen, SY, Cui, H, Zhou, RL, and Jia, YY. Revision of Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS). Chin J Clin Psychol (2012) 20(2):148–51. doi:10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2012.02.024
52. Gross, JJ, and John, OP. Individual Differences in Two Emotion Regulation Processes: Implications for Affect, Relationships, and Well-Being. J Pers Soc Psychol (2003) 85(2):348–62. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348
53. Shi, N. The Relationship Among Adult Attachment, Emotion Regulation and Interpersonal Relationship of College Students. Jining: Qufu Normal University (2012).
54. Hayes, AF. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-based Approach (3rd Edition) (2022). New York: The Guilford Press.
55. Aiken, LS, and West, SG. Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. Newbury Park: Sage Publications Inc (1991).
56. Wu, Y. Investigation on Mental Health Status in Postgraduate Freshmen. China J Health Psychol (2012) 20(1):398–400. doi:10.13342/j.cnki.cjhp.2012.01.040
57. Zhang, D, Huebner, ES, and Tian, L. Longitudinal Associations Among Neuroticism, Depression, and Cyberbullying in Early Adolescents. Comput Hum Behav (2020) 112:106475. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2020.106475
58. Liu, C, Chen, L, and Chen, S. Influence of Neuroticism on Depressive Symptoms Among Chinese Adolescents: The Mediation Effects of Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies. Front Psychiatry (2020) 11:420. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00420
59. Huang, ME, and Guo, DJ. The Influence of Extraversion and Neuroticism on Emotions. Psychol Sci (2003) 26(6):1047–51. doi:10.3969/j.issn.1671-6981.2003.06.021
60. Costa, PT, and McCrae, RR. Influence of Extraversion and Neuroticism on Subjective Well-Being: Happy and Unhappy People. J Pers Soc Psychol (1980) 38(4):668–78. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.38.4.668
61. Fetterman, AK, Robinson, MD, Ode, S, and Gordon, KH. Neuroticism as a Risk Factor for Behavioral Dysregulation: A Mindfulness-Mediation Perspective. J Soc Clin Psychol (2010) 29(3):301–21. doi:10.1521/jscp.2010.29.3.301
62. Hou, LL, Jiang, Q, Wang, HZ, and Li, CR. The Relationship between Trait Anger and Aggressive Behavior: Based on the Perspective of the Integrative Cognitive Model. Acta Psychologica Sinica (2017) 49(12):1548–58. doi:10.3724/sp.j.1041.2017.01548
63. Hasenkamp, W, Wilson-Mendenhall, CD, Duncan, E, and Barsalou, LW. Mind Wandering and Attention during Focused Meditation: a fine-grained Temporal Analysis of Fluctuating Cognitive States. Neuroimage (2012) 59(1):750–60. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.008
65. de Vibe, M, Solhaug, I, Tyssen, R, Friborg, O, Rosenvinge, JH, Sørlie, T, et al. Does Personality Moderate the Effects of Mindfulness Training for Medical and Psychology Students? Mindfulness (2015) 6(2):281–9. doi:10.1007/s12671-013-0258-y
66. Nyklíček, I, and Irrmischer, M. For Whom Does Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction Work? Moderating Effects of Personality. Mindfulness (2017) 8(4):1106–16. doi:10.1007/s12671-017-0687-0
67. Jagielski, CH, Tucker, DC, Dalton, SO, Mrug, S, Würtzen, H, and Johansen, C. Personality as a Predictor of Well-Being in a Randomized Trial of a Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction of Danish Women with Breast Cancer. J Psychosoc Oncol (2020) 38(1):4–19. doi:10.1080/07347332.2019.1626524
68. Querstret, D, Morison, L, Dickinson, S, Cropley, M, and John, M. Mindfulness-based Stress Reduction and Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for Psychological Health and Well-Being in Nonclinical Samples: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Int J Stress Manag (2020) 27(4):394–411. doi:10.1037/str0000165
Keywords: depression, neuroticism, dispositional mindfulness, cognitive reappraisal, moderated mediation model
Citation: Hou L, Chang L, Chen L, Fei J and Zhou R (2022) Exploring the Roles of Dispositional Mindfulness and Cognitive Reappraisal in the Relationship Between Neuroticism and Depression Among Postgraduate Students in China. Int J Public Health 67:1605074. doi: 10.3389/ijph.2022.1605074
Received: 20 May 2022; Accepted: 12 August 2022;
Published: 25 August 2022.
Edited by:
Nino Kuenzli, Swiss Tropical and Public Health Institute (Swiss TPH), SwitzerlandCopyright © 2022 Hou, Chang, Chen, Fei and Zhou. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Renlai Zhou, rlzhou@nju.edu.cn