- 1Centre for Health Equity Studies, Department of Public Health Sciences, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
- 2Unit for Mental Health, Children and Youth, Public Health Agency of Sweden, Stockholm, Sweden
- 3Department of Public Health and Community Medicine, Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
Objectives: Group-level characteristics in shared contexts such as schools may affect adolescent psychological health. This study examined if the immigrant density in the classroom was associated with the level of self-reported psychological complaints among students with an immigration background.
Methods: Cross-sectional data were derived from 2,619 lower secondary school students (aged 13–15) in the 2017/18 wave of the Swedish Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) survey. Using multilevel analysis estimating two-level random intercept linear regression models, classroom immigrant density was considered as a potential predictor of immigrant students’ psychological complaints.
Results: Students with an immigration background reported significantly fewer psychological complaints, on average, than students without such a background, even when adjusting for other sociodemographic characteristics. A cross-level interaction indicated that both first- and second-generation immigrant students experienced fewer psychological complaints in immigrant-dense classes compared to when the proportion of immigrant students was lower.
Conclusion: Students with an immigration background fare better psychologically in classes with a higher proportion of immigrant students. Such compositional effects could be alleviated by strengthening all schools’ capacities to provide a more inclusive classroom climate.
Introduction
The large-scale immigration experienced by countries like Sweden in the past decades has been accompanied by an increase in ethnic residential and school segregation [1]. Exposure to such structural inequalities, family socioeconomic deprivation [2] as well as challenges in negotiating identity and acculturation [3] has raised concerns about the social integration and psychological well-being of children and adolescents with an immigration background [4].
Depending on their reason for migration, country of origin, and education level, immigrant families encounter different obstacles to their mental health and well-being [5]. For instance, children of asylum seekers are considered to be at higher risk of poor psychological health, due to stressors related to displacement and resettlement [6]. Also, more generally, children and adolescents with an immigration background have been found to have higher rates of clinically diagnosed mental disorders [4]. Similar associations may apply to psychological problems more broadly. A meta-analysis of European studies showed that on average, immigrant children and youth present with higher levels of internalizing outcomes such as psychological distress and depressive symptoms than natives, especially at earlier stages of development [7].
Yet, other studies have found that children of immigrants in European countries like Sweden and elsewhere present with equal or better self-rated psychological well-being than their native peers [8]. This proposed advantage has partly been attributed to protective factors related to family conditions [8, 9]. Less attention has been placed on mechanisms related to group-level characteristics in shared contexts such as schools [10], which may promote student well-being by creating a positive psychosocial environment and providing social resources [11].
Children and adolescents’ well-being is shaped by factors in a variety of contexts. Ideological and institutional patterns are elements related to the macrosystem that can be decisive for people’s quality of life and future opportunities, for instance through social policies, while the family and the school are examples of spaces in the microsystem that play a role for young people’s outcomes [12]. As parental influence weakens during the transitional period of adolescence, peers tend to take on a more dominant role for the individual’s behavior and development [13]. Thus, peer relationships at school are essential for adolescents’ sense of belonging and level of well-being [14]. The social school environment may be particularly decisive for the well-being and future opportunities of students with an immigration background, as school presents an essential setting for sociocultural integration and interaction with majority group peers [15].
Increasing residential segregation and the “cream skimming” of children from high-SES families and the most motivated students into the most reputable schools through universal school choice has given rise to an uneven distribution of immigrant students into Swedish schools [1]. This has activated concerns about potentially harmful compositional effects. For instance, immigrant-dense schools have been suggested to negatively affect student academic achievement [16] and increase the risk of victimization [17], particularly for minority group students themselves [18]. Further, such environments may undermine successful acculturation and the development of a sense of belonging in the host society [19]. As a result, policy makers and educators alike tend to advocate for desegregation, with the aim of enhancing educational equity and student outcomes [20].
However, school characteristics that promote academic achievement may not be equally beneficial for adolescent psychological well-being [21, 22]. Attending schools and classes with a high proportion of native and/or more socioeconomically privileged peers could in fact be unfavorable for low-SES and immigrant students [23–25]. Accordingly, some studies have found that students with an immigration background report worse psychological well-being in schools with a majority of native students [26, 27]. One pathway explaining such an association may relate to social comparison mechanisms. Students from socially more disadvantaged backgrounds could feel academically inferior in high-SES schools that tend to have higher average academic achievement [28], in line with the “big-fish-little-pond” effect [29]. Experiencing a more competitive academic environment can also be expected to produce more stress related to school performance, which has in turn been associated with lower levels of psychological well-being [30].
Moreover, for adolescents with an immigration background, the theory of a protective ethnic density effect, established in the context of neighborhood influences on health [31], may apply in the school setting [32]. Ethnic minority populations have been shown to benefit from living in residential areas with a higher concentration of ethnic minority people [31], conceivably due to stronger social relationships [33] and less exposure to stigma related to their status as a minority [34]. In line with such theories, one potential mechanism accounting for the link between classroom immigrant density and the psychological well-being of immigrant students may be a stronger sense of immediate belonging and acceptance by peers [14].
During the developmental phase of adolescence, peers act as social mirrors, validating each other’s self-image [35]. Consequently, the affirmative experience of acceptance by peers in a central social setting such as the school can be decisive for the individual’s self-esteem [36], which is in turn linked with psychological well-being [37]. Students with an immigration background, particularly those with a more visible minority status, could find it more difficult to acquire group membership and a sense of belonging in classrooms with a high proportion of majority group students, as they may face more stigma related to their background in these contexts [38]. Such rejection by classmates in the school setting can be damaging for an adolescent’s self-image, and thus contribute to lower psychological well-being among immigrant students [26, 39]. By contrast, when surrounded by a high proportion of other students who are foreign-born or have foreign-born parents, students with an immigration background might have better chances of experiencing a stronger sense of belonging, in turn generating a higher level of well-being.
With such mechanisms in mind, the objective of this study was to test if (i) students with an immigration background generally reported less psychological complaints than students without an immigration background and if so (ii) to examine if this association was moderated by classroom immigrant density.
Methods
Study Sample
The study used data from the 2017/18 Swedish Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) study, a cross-sectional survey among 11-, 13-, and 15-year-olds that is part of an international World Health Organization collaborative study. The survey was conducted in grades 5, 7, and 9, with schools being randomly selected from all public and independent schools in Sweden for each grade, where after one class per school was randomly sampled. The questionnaires were distributed anonymously to all students present in class on the day of the survey.
As this study focused on lower secondary school, only students in grades 7 and 9 were included (n = 3,095 distributed across 145 school classes). The final study sample was restricted to students with complete information for the relevant variables (n = 2,619 distributed over 145 school classes).
In the data, 239 students attended schools where no participating students reported having an immigration background. As a sensitivity check (not presented in table), we ran all analyses excluding these students. The results from these additional analyses remained almost identical to those presented.
Measures
Psychological complaints were operationalized as an index of four items. Participants indicated how frequently they experienced each of the following symptoms: feeling low, feeling nervous, irritability or bad temper, and difficulties in getting to sleep. Four response options were provided for each question: “about every day”, “more than once a week”, “about every week”, “about every month”, and “rarely or never.” A summary scale with a range of 4–20 was developed, with a higher score indicating a higher frequency of psychological complaints. A Canadian study that examined the external construct validity of the HBSC psychological health self-report question series found that the four items included in the measure of psychological complaints used in this study presented as one single factor [40].
Immigration background differentiated between students who reported being born abroad and/or having two parents who were born abroad, and students without an immigration background. A more refined version of this variable further differentiated between first- (students born abroad) and second-generation immigrant students (students born in Sweden with two foreign-born parents).
Classroom immigrant density was measured as the share of students with immigration background in the class in per cent.
The analyses were adjusted by several control variables. The Family Affluence Scale (FAS) is a measure reflecting family expenditure and consumption used to identify the socioeconomic status of adolescents. This study used version III of FAS [41], an index consisting of six items: family car ownership; having one’s own bedroom; number of computers (including laptops and tablets) in the household; number of bathrooms in the household; ownership of dishwasher; number of times travelled abroad on holiday in last year. Responses to the items were summed up and calculated as an index ranging from 0–13.
Family structure differentiated between students who explicitly reported living with both parents (coded 1) and those who alternated between two parents, lived with only one parent, or in any other circumstances (coded 0).
We further controlled for school grade (year 7 or 9) and student gender (boy or girl).
Statistical Analysis
Analyses were performed using Stata version 14.2 (StataCorp LLC, College Station, TX, United States). The hierarchical nature of the data with students nested in school classes warranted a multilevel approach [42] A series of two-level random intercept linear regression models were run using the “mixed” command, examining the role of immigration background and classroom immigrant density for psychological complaints. Model 1 estimated the level of psychological complaints of students with an immigration background compared to those without such a background, controlling for school grade, student gender, family structure and socioeconomic status. Model 2 further adjusted for classroom immigrant density. Model 3 added a cross-level interaction between student immigration background and classroom immigrant density. Intra Class Correlation coefficients (ICC), indicating the amount of variation that can be attributed to the higher level, were reported for all models.
Ethical Considerations
The Swedish HBSC data does not contain any personal identification and the questionnaire is completed anonymously and voluntarily by the participants. Informed consent was obtained from the students who participated. Parents/guardians were informed about the study by the schools and parent who did not want their children to participate were asked to inform the school. The collection of data with the Swedish HBSC survey was not classified as personal data and thus this study was deemed exempt from human subjects review by the Regional Ethical Review Board in Stockholm, Sweden.
Results
As shown in Table 1, immigrant students reported significantly fewer psychological complaints, on average (10.21), than students without an immigration background (11.04). Further, students with an immigration background had a significantly lower family socioeconomic status (14.61) than students without an immigration background (15.69), on average. The average immigrant density of a student’s respective school class was significantly higher for immigration background students (46.70) than for students without an immigration background (17.49). While 38.6% of immigrant students were part of classes with an immigrant density of at least 50%, the corresponding share for students without an immigration background was 4.0% (data not presented). Finally, a significantly larger proportion of students without an immigration background reported living with both parents (69.8%) than among students with an immigration background (65.3%).
TABLE 1. Descriptive statistics of the variables for the total study sample and by immigration background (based on 2,619 students distributed over 145 school classes). Health Behavior in School-aged Children study, Sweden, 2017/18.
Table 2 presents results from random intercept models with psychological complaints as the dependent variable.
TABLE 2. Results from two-level random intercept linear regression models (b coefficients). Student-reported psychological complaints according to immigration background (based on 2,619 students distributed across 145 school classes). Health Behavior in School-aged Children study, Sweden, 2017/18.
The intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) for Model 1 shows that only 3.8% of the variance in adolescents’ psychological complaints is accounted for by school-level, rather than student-level, differences (ICC = 0.038), indicating that there is generally a rather low similarity between students from the same school with regards to psychological complaints.
As a first step, differences in levels of psychological complaints between immigrant and non-immigrant students were reviewed (Model 1). Even when adjusting for school grade, gender, family structure and socioeconomic status, students with an immigration background reported significantly fewer psychological complaints (b = −0.941, p ≤ 0.001, 95% CI −1.31, −0.57) on average, than students without an immigration background. This advantage was lowered to −0.907 (p ≤ 0.001, 95% CI −1.32, −0.50) when further adjusting for classroom proportion of immigrant students, although this class-level measure did not show a statistically significant association with psychological complaints (b = −0.002, p = 0.692, 95% CI −0.01, 0.01) (Model 2). Next, a cross-level interaction between individual immigration background and classroom immigrant density was added (Model 3). This interaction term was negative and statistically significant (b = −0.023, p = 0.006, 95% CI −0.04, −0.007), conveying that the association between individual student immigration background and the level of psychological complaints varied based on the proportion of immigrant students in the classroom. More specifically, this result demonstrates that students with an immigration background experienced fewer psychological complaints when in classes with a higher proportion of immigrant students compared to being in classes with a low immigrant density.
Table 3 presents the same models as in Table 2 while using a more refined version of the independent variable, distinguishing between first- and second-generation immigrants. Both first- (b = −1.08, p ≤ 0.001, 95% CI −1.57, −0.60) and second-generation immigrants (b = −0.83, p ≤ 0.001, 95% CI −1.32, −0.34) reported lower levels of psychological complaints, on average, than students without any immigration background (Model 1). These associations were slightly reduced for both first- (b = −1.05, p ≤ 0.001, 95% CI −1.55, −0.54) and second-generation (b = −0.78, p ≤ 0.001, 95% CI −1.31, −0.25) immigrant groups in Model 2. The cross-level interaction was also statistically significant for both immigrant background groups, suggesting that the finding regarding the role of class immigrant density applies to both first- (b = −0.021, p = 0.037, 95% CI −0.04, −0.001) and second-generation immigrants (b = −0.026, p = 0.008, 95% CI −0.05, −0.007).
TABLE 3. Results from two-level random intercept linear regression models (b coefficients). Student-reported psychological complaints according to first- and second-generation immigrant status (based on 2,612a students distributed across 145 school classes). Health Behavior in School-aged Children study, Sweden, 2017/18.
Discussion
The results of the current study corroborate previous research that has suggested an advantage in self-reported psychological well-being among children of immigrants in Sweden [8]. With respect to immigrant density, the study found that adolescents with an immigration background experienced fewer psychological complaints when in school classes with a higher proportion of other immigrant students than in classes with a majority of non-immigrant students. Although these findings are not able to account for the levels of psychological well-being of specific groups of immigrants, such as children of refugees [6], these associations were shown to be of relevance for both first- and second-generation immigrants.
The findings regarding the association between classroom immigrant density and the level of student psychological complaints are in line with a previous Swedish study by Hjern et al. [27] which showed that specifically migrant students born in Africa or Asia reported more positive psychological well-being in classes with a high density of students with a migrant origin. Similar mechanisms that account for group density effects in communities at large [31] may thus be relevant for adolescents in schools.
The examination of immigrant density in schools is particularly relevant considering the extensive immigration to countries like Sweden in the past decade [43], which has exacerbated the spatial segregation of different sociodemographic groups and the corresponding uneven distribution of children into schools [1]. As reflected in the most recent Swedish HBSC data, a large proportion of immigrant students are indeed likely to attend schools with a high immigrant density. Notwithstanding, some immigrant families choose to place their children in schools with a majority of native students that tend to be perceived as more prestigious, at times in search of a more advantageous “Swedish” learning environment [44].
Yet, having an immigration background may render students more vulnerable to social exclusion in such school settings, as previous studies have indicated. For instance, a Swedish study by Plenty and Jonsson [38] found that students with an immigration background had higher peer rejection rates than native students, particularly in classes with a low immigrant density. Likewise, another study based on Swedish data showed that students born in Africa and Asia attending classes with few other immigrants were at high risk of being bullied [27]. Such negative judgements by peers, signaling low acceptance, can in turn be decisive for an individual’s self-esteem and psychological well-being. Thus, while attending school with a high proportion of majority group students could potentially be advantageous for immigrant students in terms of social integration, the acquisition of the local language and improved academic achievement [45], they may struggle more to find a sense of acceptance and belonging than in classes with a higher proportion of fellow immigrants. As a consequence of the deeply rooted need to be accepted by peers [36], the existing segregated school landscape in Sweden may thus discourage adolescents with an immigration background to apply to schools with a lower immigrant density, further reinforcing school segregation and undermining the integration into the majority society.
Aside from processes related to peer acceptance, there are other micro-level mechanisms that may account for the proposed association between immigrant density and psychological complaints among immigration background students. For instance, some students with an immigration background could find themselves at an academic disadvantage when compared to native classmates, particularly those students who have recently immigrated and/or have a less socioeconomically privileged family background. Further, students with a more socially disadvantaged background may not be able to fully benefit academically from a relatively more effective learning context when attending more socially privileged schools, as these schools may not always provide appropriate academic and psychosocial support to such student groups. Thus, social comparison mechanisms could have negative implications for the academic self-concept of immigrant students in schools with a high proportion of native Swedish students [28], with potentially adverse short- and long-term effects on well-being.
Ultimately, the findings of this study suggest that the transfer of individual immigrant students to majority-dense schools, for instance through the universal school voucher system established in Sweden [1, 44], may not necessarily benefit the immediate psychological well-being of this group of adolescents, despite potential academic benefits [21]. However, even if adolescents with an immigration background may present with fewer psychological complaints when they attend schools and classes with many other immigrant students, this may not be an indication of equally positive outcomes in other important areas such as educational achievements and employment prospects. Here, factors at the macro-level such as housing segregation must be acknowledged as challenges that undermine the successful integration of immigrant families and may end up standing in the way of adolescents’ future opportunities.
Limitations
The use of HBSC data provides a valuable opportunity to map patterns of adolescent health and well-being. However, due to the cross-sectional nature of the HBSC surveys, we cannot draw any causal conclusions based on our findings. Further, some shortcomings of this study have limited the scope of our analyses. Firstly, as mentioned in the Introduction, research findings on the relative psychological well-being of children and adolescents with an immigration background are divided, and several studies have determined that immigrant children generally fare worse than children of natives [7]. For instance, children of certain groups of immigrants, such as asylum-seekers, are known to be at high risk for mental and psychosocial problems [46]. However, the Swedish HBSC survey does not provide information on country of origin, time since immigration, or the reasons for migration, so that it was not possible to uncover potential groups of immigrants that could be driving the established relationship [27]. Additional analyses (not presented in table) that distinguished between immigrants of European and non-European origin showed that classroom immigrant density was statistically significantly associated with psychological complaints for students of non-European origin, but not for those with a European background However, since only 2.5% (n = 66) of our sample reported having a background in another European country, we opted to merge this group with students of non-European origin. Furthermore, records of student achievement or ability would have been useful as control or potential mediation variables. Finally, the outcome variable psychological complaints was operationalized as a summary scale, thus not accounting for the categorical nature of the individual items. Further, it should be stressed that the outcome variable captures self-reported symptoms that do not reflect clinical diagnoses, implying that the identified advantage in psychological well-being among adolescents with an immigration background cannot be generalized to the prevalence of more serious psychiatric disorders that may require treatment [47].
Conclusion
This study investigated the potential role of classroom immigrant density for immigrant students’ psychological complaints. The results show that adolescents in Sweden who were foreign-born or had two foreign-born parents reported fewer psychological complaints when in school classes with a higher immigrant density than in classes with a low proportion of immigrant students. This association were shown to apply to both first- and second-generation immigrant students.
The findings suggest that efforts to cultivate a more inclusive social classroom climate may be able to mitigate compositional effects and thus promote the psychological well-being of adolescents with an immigration background, even in a segregated school landscape. Accordingly, policy makers and schools should become more aware of class compositional effects that may affect the general psychological well-being of students with an immigration background. While the distribution of students into schools is determined by factors at the macro-level and thus largely beyond the control of schools themselves, an increased awareness of the importance of feeling accepted in the classroom, particularly for minorities, is vital for the successful integration and academic attainment of all students. Thus, all schools should be provided with resources and tools to develop an inclusive academic and social climate where students are less likely to be excluded or stigmatized based on their ethnic or immigration background [15, 20] Yet, structural responses may be required to break current segregation patterns and to improve future opportunities for children of immigrants.
To reinforce such objectives, future studies should explore evidence regarding potential mechanisms that could account for the association between the sociodemographic classroom composition and student well-being. For instance, certain school and classroom characteristics could have a buffering effect that could improve the sense of belonging of immigrant students, even in majority-dense classrooms. It may also be valuable to observe the development of the significance of immigrant density in schools over time, following the recent inflow of migrants and refugees in the past few years. Finally, there is a need for more detailed studies of different groups of immigrants, for example based on country or region of origin, in order to ensure that students who may be particularly vulnerable to exclusion and victimization at school can be acknowledged and effectively supported.
Data Availability Statement
The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: Data can be requested from the Public Health Agency of Sweden. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to cGV0cmEubG9mc3RlZHRAZm9sa2hhbHNvbXluZGlnaGV0ZW4uc2U=.
Author Contributions
MS conceived the study, participated in its design and coordination, performed the statistical analysis and drafted the manuscript. SL and PL critically reviewed and revised drafts of the paper. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Funding
The study was conducted within the research project “Health Behaviour in School-aged Children: Individual and contextual associations with psychological (ill-)health” funded by the Public Health Agency of Sweden.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the Public Health Agency of Sweden for granting us access to the data that made this study possible.
References
1. Böhlmark, A, Holmlund, H, and Lindahl, M. Parental Choice, Neighbourhood Segregation or Cream Skimming? an Analysis of School Segregation after a Generalized Choice Reform. J Popul Econ (2016) 29(4):1155–90. doi:10.1007/s00148-016-0595-y
2. Gustafsson, B, and Österberg, T. How Are Immigrant Children in Sweden Faring? Mean Income, Affluence and Poverty since the 1980s. Child Ind Res (2018) 11(1):329–53. doi:10.1007/s12187-016-9416-9
3. Vedder, P, and Virta, E. Language, Ethnic Identity, and the Adaptation of Turkish Immigrant Youth in the Netherlands and Sweden. Int J Intercultural Relations (2005) 29(3):317–37. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2005.05.006
4. Kouider, EB, Koglin, U, and Petermann, F. Emotional and Behavioral Problems in Migrant Children and Adolescents in Europe: a Systematic Review. Eur Child Adoles Psy (2014) 23(6):373–91.
5. Borraccino, A, Charrier, L, Berchialla, P, Lazzeri, G, Vieno, A, Dalmasso, P, et al. Perceived Well-Being in Adolescent Immigrants: it Matters where They Come from. Int J Public Health (2018) 63(9):1037–45. doi:10.1007/s00038-018-1165-8
6. Fazel, M, Reed, RV, Panter-Brick, C, and Stein, A. Mental Health of Displaced and Refugee Children Resettled in High-Income Countries: Risk and Protective Factors. The Lancet (2012) 379(9812):266–82. doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(11)60051-2
7. Dimitrova, R, Chasiotis, A, and Van de Vijver, F. Adjustment Outcomes of Immigrant Children and Youth in Europe. Eur Psychol (2016) 21(2):150–62. doi:10.1027/1016-9040/a000246
8. Mood, C, Jonsson, JO, and Låftman, SB. The Mental Health Advantage of Immigrant-Background Youth: The Role of Family Factors. Fam Relat (2017) 79(2):419–36. doi:10.1111/jomf.12340
9. Harding, S, Read, UM, Molaodi, OR, Cassidy, A, Maynard, MJ, Lenguerrand, E, et al. The Determinants of Young Adult Social Well-Being and Health (DASH) Study: Diversity, Psychosocial Determinants and Health. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol (2015) 50(8):1173–88. doi:10.1007/s00127-015-1047-9
10. Marsh, HW, Lüdtke, O, Nagengast, B, Trautwein, U, Morin, AJS, Abduljabbar, AS, et al. Classroom Climate and Contextual Effects: Conceptual and Methodological Issues in the Evaluation of Group-Level Effects. Educ Psychol (2012) 47(2):106–24. doi:10.1080/00461520.2012.670488
11. Sonmark, K, and Modin, B. Psychosocial Work Environment in School and Students' Somatic Health Complaints: An Analysis of Buffering Resources. Scand J Public Health (2017) 45(1):64–72. doi:10.1177/1403494816677116
12. Bronfenbrenner, U, and Morris, PA. The Ecology of Developmental Processes. In: Damon, W, editor. Handbook of Child Psychology, 1. New York: John Wiley & Sons (1998). p. 993–1028.
13. Brown, BB, and Larson, J. Peer Relationships in Adolescence. In: Lerner, RM,, and Steinberg, L, editors. Handbook of Adolescent Psychology. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons (2009). p. 74–103.
14. Chiu, MM, Chow, BW-Y, McBride, C, and Mol, ST. Students' Sense of Belonging at School in 41 Countries. J Cross-Cultural Psychol (2016) 47(2):175–96. doi:10.1177/0022022115617031
15. Horenczyk, G, and Tatar, M. Conceptualising the School Acculturative Context. School, Classroom and the Immigrant Student”. In: Masten, AS, Liebkind K, and Hernandez, DJ, editors. Realizing the Potential of Immigrant Youth. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press (2012). p. 359–75.
16. Jensen, P, and Rasmussen, AW. The Effect of Immigrant Concentration in Schools on Native and Immigrant Children's reading and Math Skills. Econ Edu Rev (2011) 30(6):1503–15. doi:10.1016/j.econedurev.2011.08.002
17. Walsh, SD, De Clercq, B, Molcho, M, Harel-Fisch, Y, Davison, CM, Rich Madsen, K, et al. The Relationship between Immigrant School Composition, Classmate Support and Involvement in Physical Fighting and Bullying Among Adolescent Immigrants and Non-immigrants in 11 Countries. J Youth Adolescence (2016) 45(1):1–16. doi:10.1007/s10964-015-0367-0
18. Brunello, G, and De Paola, M. School Segregation of Immigrants and its Effects on Educational Outcomes in Europe. Luxembourg: Publications Office of the European Union (2017).
19. Lee, S. Does Identity Matter for Adaptation? the Influence of Acculturation Identity on the Well-Being and Delinquency of Adolescent Students in Europe. J Youth Stud (2020) 23(6):797–817. doi:10.1080/13676261.2019.1636950
20. Thijs, J, and Verkuyten, M. School Ethnic Diversity and Students' Interethnic Relations. Br J Educ Psychol (2014) 84(1):1–21. doi:10.1111/bjep.12032
21. Granvik Saminathen, M, Låftman, SB, and Modin, B. School Choice at a Cost? Academic Achievement, School Satisfaction and Psychological Complaints Among Students in Disadvantaged Areas of Stockholm. Ijerph (2019) 16(11):1912. doi:10.3390/ijerph16111912
22. Opdenakker, M-C, and Van Damme, J. Effects of Schools, Teaching Staff and Classes on Achievement and Well-Being in Secondary Education: Similarities and Differences between School Outcomes. Sch Effectiveness Sch Improvement (2000) 11(2):165–96. doi:10.1076/0924-3453(200006)11:2;1-q;ft165
23. Ackert, E. Segregation Paradox? School Racial/ethnic and Socioeconomic Composition and Racial/ethnic Differences in Engagement. Soc Sci Res (2018) 70:144–62. doi:10.1016/j.ssresearch.2017.10.010
24. Hornstra, L, van der Veen, I, Peetsma, T, and Volman, M. Does Classroom Composition Make a Difference: Effects on Developments in Motivation, Sense of Classroom Belonging, and Achievement in Upper Primary School. Sch Effectiveness Sch Improvement (2015) 26(2):125–52. doi:10.1080/09243453.2014.887024
25. Juvonen, J, Kogachi, K, and Graham, S. When and How Do Students Benefit from Ethnic Diversity in Middle School? Child Dev (2018) 89(4):1268–82. doi:10.1111/cdev.12834
26. Gieling, M, Vollebergh, W, and van Dorsselaer, S. Ethnic Density in School Classes and Adolescent Mental Health. Soc Psychiat Epidemiol (2010) 45(6):639–46. doi:10.1007/s00127-009-0105-6
27. Hjern, A, Rajmil, L, Bergström, M, Berlin, M, Gustafsson, PA, and Modin, B. Migrant Density and Well-Being-A National School Survey of 15-Year-Olds in Sweden. Eur J Public Health (2013) 23(5):823–8. doi:10.1093/eurpub/ckt106
28. Göllner, R, Damian, RI, Nagengast, B, Roberts, BW, and Trautwein, U. It's Not Only Who You Are but Who You Are with: High School Composition and Individuals' Attainment over the Life Course. Psychol Sci (2018) 29(11):1785–96. doi:10.1177/0956797618794454
29. Roy, A, Guay, F, and Valois, P. The Big-Fish-Little-Pond Effect on Academic Self-Concept: The Moderating Role of Differentiated Instruction and Individual Achievement. Learn Individual Differences (2015) 42:110–6. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2015.07.009
30. Heller-Sahlgren, G. Smart but Unhappy: Independent-School Competition and the Wellbeing-Efficiency Trade-Off in Education. Econ Edu Rev (2018) 62:66–81. doi:10.1016/j.econedurev.2017.10.005
31. Bécares, L, Dewey, ME, and Das-Munshi, J. Ethnic Density Effects for Adult Mental Health: Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of International Studies. Psychol Med (2018) 48(12):2054–72. doi:10.1017/s0033291717003580
32. Pickett, KE, and Wilkinson, RG. People like Us: Ethnic Group Density Effects on Health. Ethn Health (2008) 13(4):321–34. doi:10.1080/13557850701882928
33. Stansfeld, SA. Social Support and Social Cohesion. In: Marmot, M,, and Wilkinson, RG, editors. Social Determinants of Health. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2006). p. 148–71.
34. Link, BG, and Phelan, JC. Stigma and its Public Health Implications. The Lancet (2006) 367(9509):528–9. doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(06)68184-1
35. Gifford-Smith, ME, and Brownell, CA. Childhood Peer Relationships: Social Acceptance, Friendships, and Peer Networks. J Sch Psychol (2003) 41(4):235–84. doi:10.1016/s0022-4405(03)00048-7
36. Tetzner, J, Becker, M, and Maaz, K. Development in Multiple Areas of Life in Adolescence. Int J Behav Dev (2017) 41(6):704–13. doi:10.1177/0165025416664432
37. Orth, U, Robins, RW, and Roberts, BW. Low Self-Esteem Prospectively Predicts Depression in Adolescence and Young Adulthood. J Personal Soc Psychol (2008) 95(3):695–708. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.3.695
38. Plenty, S, and Jonsson, JO. Social Exclusion Among Peers: The Role of Immigrant Status and Classroom Immigrant Density. J Youth Adolescence (2017) 46(6):1275–88. doi:10.1007/s10964-016-0564-5
39. Tummala-Narra, P, and Claudius, M. Perceived Discrimination and Depressive Symptoms Among Immigrant-Origin Adolescents. Cult Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol (2013) 19(3):257–69. doi:10.1037/a0032960
40. Gariepy, G, McKinnon, B, Sentenac, M, and Elgar, FJ. Validity and Reliability of a Brief Symptom Checklist to Measure Psychological Health in School-Aged Children. Child Ind Res (2016) 9(2):471–84. doi:10.1007/s12187-015-9326-2
41. Torsheim, T, Cavallo, F, Cavallo, F, Levin, KA, Schnohr, C, Mazur, J, et al. Psychometric Validation of the Revised Family Affluence Scale: a Latent Variable Approach. Child Ind Res (2016) 9(3):771–84. doi:10.1007/s12187-015-9339-x
42. Rasbash, J, Steele, F, Browne, WJ, and Goldstein, H. A User’s Guide to MLwiN: Version 2.32. Bristol: Centre for Multilevel Modelling, University of Bristol (2015).
43. Kowalczyk, JA. Schools, Risk, and Integration: Responding to the EU/refugee Crisis. Eur Edu (2018) 50:1–5. doi:10.1080/10564934.2018.1431508
44. Bunar, N. The Controlled School Market and Urban Schools in Sweden. J Sch Choice (2010) 4(1):47–73. doi:10.1080/15582151003626418
45. Benito, R, Alegre, MÁ, and González-Balletbó, I. School Segregation and its Effects on Educational equality and Efficiency in 16 OECD Comprehensive School Systems. Comp Educ Rev (2013) 58(1):104–34.
46. World Health Organization, . Health of Refugee and Migrant Children. Technical Guidance. Copenhagen: WHO Regional Office for Europe (2018).
47. Bremberg, S, and Dalman, C. Begrepp, Mätmetoder Och Förekomst Av Psykisk Hälsa, Psykisk Ohälsa Och Psykiatriska Tillstånd Hos Barn Och Unga: En Kunskapsöversikt [Concepts, Measurement and Prevalence of Psychological Health, Illness and Psychiatric Conditions Among Children and Youth: An Overview]. Stockholm, Sweden: Forte (2015).
Keywords: immigrant density, psychological well-being, adolescence, Sweden, school
Citation: Granvik Saminathen M, Löfstedt P and Låftman SB (2021) Classroom Immigrant Density Predicts Psychological Well-Being Among Adolescents With an Immigration Background: Findings From the 2017/18 Swedish Health Behaviour in School-Aged Children Study. Int J Public Health 66:647380. doi: 10.3389/ijph.2021.647380
Received: 29 December 2020; Accepted: 02 July 2021;
Published: 19 July 2021.
Edited by:
Lyda Osorio, University of Valle, ColombiaCopyright © 2021 Granvik Saminathen, Löfstedt and Låftman. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Maria Granvik Saminathen, bWlhLmdyYW52aWtAc3Uuc2U=